Advertisement

Research on Emotions and Future Developments in Human Ethology

  • Glenn Weisfeld

Abstract

This chapter presents a model of the basic human emotions which, it is argued, can be taken as comprising the main elements of the human ethogram and hence of human behavior. The chapter begins with a critical review of criteria that have commonly been used in constructing such lists, and argues for heavy reliance on the criteria of adaptive value and affect. Since affect is the most characteristic feature of emotions and seems to induce their particular behavioral tendencies, expressions, and visceral adjustments, affect is perhaps the single best defining feature of each emotional modality. Because of its ethological nature, this sort of list of emotions may be more comprehensive than alternative lists lacking a comparative basis, and it avoids superficial dichotomies between types of emotions. Some general properties of affects are then discussed, such as their valence, intensity, and timing. Some possible applications of this model for studying a wide range of human behaviors are then suggested, including sex and developmental differences, and individual, cultural, and pathological variation. It is argued that ethological methods and a phylogenetic perspective are essential for arriving at a complete description of the human emotions and their facets, while at the same time avoiding an overly cybernetic modularization model.

Keywords

Parental Behavior Emotional Expression Basic Emotion Human Emotion Observational Research 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Alexander, R. D. (1986). Ostracism and indirect reciprocity: the reproductive significance of humor. Ethology and Sociobiology, 7, 253–270.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Apicella, P., Ljungberg, T. Scarnati, E., & Schultz, W. (1991). Responses to Reward in Monkey Doral and Ventral Striatum. Experimental Brain Research, 85, 491–500.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Baker, J. W., & Schaie, K. W. (1969). Effects of aggression “alone” or with “another” on physiological and psychological arousal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 12, 80–96.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Barber, N. (1991). Play and energy regulation in mammals. Quarterly Review of Biology, 66, 129–147.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Becker, J.B., Breedlove, S. M., & Crews, D. (1992). Behavioral Endocrinology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  6. Blurton Jones, N. (1972). Categories of child-child interaction. In N. Blurton Jones (Ed.), Ethological Studies of Child Behaviour (pp. 97–127). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  7. Boulton, M. J., & Smith, P. K. (1992). The social nature of playfighting and play chasing: mechanisms and strategies underlying co-operation and compromise. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  8. Bramel, D., Taub, B., & Blum, B. (1968). An observer’s reaction to the suffering of his enemy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 8, 384–392.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Buck, R. (1988). Human Motivation and Emotion, second edition. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  10. Cattell, R. B. (1957). Personality and Motivation: Structure and measurement. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
  11. Charlesworth, W. R. (1969). The role of surprise in cognitive development. In D. Elkind & J. Flavell (Eds.), Studies in Cognitive Development: Essays in honor of Jean Piaget (pp. 257–314). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  12. Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ Error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Grosset/Putnam.Google Scholar
  13. Darwin, C. (1872). The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. Reprinted—New York: Philosophical Library, 1955; Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1965.Google Scholar
  14. Davis, R. C. (1934). The specificity of facial expressions. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 10, 42–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. de Waal, F. B. M. (1989). Peacemaking among Primates. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  16. Dissanayake, E. (1988). What Is Art For? Seattle: University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
  17. Dong, Q., Weisfeld, G., Boardway, R., & Shen, J. (1996). Correlates of social status among Chinese adolescents. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 27, 476–493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Dunbar, R. I. M. (1993). Coevolution of neocortical size, group size, and language in humans. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 681–735.Google Scholar
  19. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1973). The expressive behavior of the deaf-and-blind-born. In M. von Cranach, & I. Vine (Eds.), Social Communication and Movement (pp. 163–194). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  20. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1975). Ethology: The biology of behavior, 2nd ed. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
  21. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1989). Human Ethology. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
  22. Ekman, P. (Ed.) (1973). Darwin and Facial Expression. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  23. Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1986). A new pan-cultural expression of emotion. Motivation and Emotion, 10, 159–168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1971). Constants across culture in the face and emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 17, 124–129.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Ellis, L. (1986). Evidence of neuroandrogenic etiology of sex roles from a combined analysis of human, nonhuman primate and nonprimate mammalian studies. Personality and Individual Differences, 7, 519–552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Fisher, H. (1992). Anatomy of Love: The mysteries of Mating, marriage, and why we stray. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
  27. Fridlund, A. J. (1994). Human Facial Expression: An evolutionary view. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  28. Goldberg, S., Blumberg, S. L., & Kriger, A. (1982). Menarche and interest in infants: Biological and social influences. Child Development, 53, 1554–1560.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Goodall, J. (1986). The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Behavior patterns. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  30. Grammer, K., & Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (in press). The ritualization of laughter. In Naturlichkeit der Sprache und der Kultur. Bochumer Beitrage zur Semiotik. W. A. Koch (Ed.), Bochum: Brockmeyer, Vol. 18.Google Scholar
  31. Goodall, J. (1986). The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  32. Gottman, J. M. (1991). Predicting the longitudinal course of marriages. Journal of marital and Family Therapy, 17, 3–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Hess, E. H. (1965). Attitude and pupil size. Scientific American, 212, 46–54.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Hokanson, J. E. (1970). Psychophysiological evaluation of the catharsis hypothesis. In E. I. Megargee & J. E. Hokanson (Eds.), The Dynamics of Aggression (pp. 74–86). New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
  35. Hudson, J. A. (1995). Sleep. New York: Freeman.Google Scholar
  36. Hutt, C. (1972). Males and females. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
  37. Izard, C. E. (1972). Patterns of Emotions: A new analysis of anxiety and depression. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  38. Jackson, L. A. (1992). Physical Appearance and Gender: Sociobiological and Sociocultural Perspectives. Albany: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
  39. Jankowiak, W. R., & Fischer, E. F. (1992). A cross-cultural perspective on romantic love. Ethos, 31, 149–155.Google Scholar
  40. Kahn, E. S., & Weisfeld, G. (1993, August). Facial expressions that influence subjects’ postural erectness. Paper presented at the Human Behavior and Evolution Society convention, Binghamton, New York.Google Scholar
  41. Kalin, N. H., (1993). The neurobiology of fear. Scientific American, 268, 54–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Kraemer, G. W. (1992). A psychobiological theory of attachment. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 15, 493–541.Google Scholar
  43. LeDoux, J. E. (1993). Emotional networks in the brain. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of Emotions (pp. 109–118). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  44. Lewin, K., Dembo, T., Festinger, L., & Sears, P. S. (1944). Level of aspiration. In J. McV. Hunt (Ed.), Personality and the Behavior Disorders, Vol. 1. New York: Ronald.Google Scholar
  45. Lewis, M., & Michalson, L. (1983). Children’s emotions and moods. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  46. Lorenz, K. (1953). Die Entwicklung der vergleichenden Verhaltensforschung in den letzten 12 Jahren. Stud. Generale, 9, 36–58.Google Scholar
  47. MacLean, P. D. (1993). Cerebral evolution of emotion. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of Emotions (pp. 67–83). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  48. Mallick, S. K., & McCandless, B. R. (1966). A study of catharsis of aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4, 591–596.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Masters, R. D., & McGuire, M. T. (1994). The Neurotransmitter Revolution. Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.Google Scholar
  50. McDougall, W. (1923). Outline of Psychology. New York: Charles Scribner’s Sons.Google Scholar
  51. Mealey, L. (1995). The sociobiology of sociopathy: An integrated evolutionary model. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18, 523–599.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Money, J., & Ehrhardt, A. A. (1972). Man and woman, boy and girl: The differentiation and dimorphism of gender identity from conception to maturity. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
  53. Moyer, K. E. (1976). The Psychobiology of Aggression. Ne York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
  54. Oatley, K., & Jenkins, J. M. (1996). Understanding Emotions. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  55. Panksepp, J. (1994). Evolution constructed the potential for subjective experience within the neurodynamics of the neomammalian brain. In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The Nature of Emotion: Fundmental questions (pp. 396–399). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  56. Pastore, N. (1952). The role of arbitrariness in the frustration-aggression hypothesis. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 47, 727–731.Google Scholar
  57. Plomin, R. (1990). Nature and Nurture: An introduction to human behavioral genetics. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.Google Scholar
  58. Plutchik, R. (1980). Emotion: A Psychoevolutionary Synthesis. New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
  59. Plutchik, R. (1994). The Psychology and Biology of Emotion. New York: Harper Collins.Google Scholar
  60. Provine, R. R. (1989). Contagious yawning and infant imitation. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 27, 125–127.Google Scholar
  61. Pugh, G. E. (1977). The Biological Origin of Human Values. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  62. Roes, F. L. (1989). On the origin of crying and tears. Human Ethology Newsletter, 5, 5–6.Google Scholar
  63. Roes, F. L. (1993). Reactance theory and Darwinism: an example of theoretical reduction. Human Ethology Newsletter, 8, 6–7.Google Scholar
  64. Rosenzweig, M. R., Leiman, A. L., & Breedlove, S. M. (1996). Biological Psychology. Sundeland, MA: Sinauer.Google Scholar
  65. Rozin, P., & Fallon, A. E. (1987). A perspective on disgust. Psychological Review, 94, 23–41.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Russell, J. A. (1991). Confusions about context in the judgment of facial expression: A reply to “The contempt expression and the relativity thesis.” Motivation and Emotion, 15, 177–184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Russell, J. A. (1994). Is there universal recognition of emotion from facial expression? Psychological Bulletin, 115, 102–141.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Schanberg, S. M., & Field, T. M. (1987). Sensory deprivation, stress and supplemental stimulation in the rat pup and preterm human neonate. Child Development, 58, 1431–1447.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Schell, N. J., & Weisfeld, C. C. (1994, August). Marital power dynamics: A multimeasure approach. Paper presented at the conference of the International Society for Human Ethology, Toronto.Google Scholar
  70. Scott, J. P. (1958). Animal Behavior. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  71. Shimoda, K., Argyle, M., & Ricci Bitti, P. (1978). The intercultural recognition of emotional expressions by three national racial groups: English, Italian and Japanese. European Journal of Social Psychology, 8, 169–179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Sogon, S., & Masutani, M. (1989). Identification of emotion from body movement. Psychological Reports, 65, 35–46.Google Scholar
  73. Stanley, H. M. (1989). Remarks on ticklng and laughing. American Journal of Sociology, 9, 235–240.Google Scholar
  74. Steiner, J. E. (1979). Human facial expressions in response to taste and smell stimulation. In H. Reese & L. P. Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances in Child Development and Behavior (pp. 257–293). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
  75. Tinbergen, N. (1951). The Study of Instinct. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  76. Van Bezooijen, R., Van Otto, S. A., & Heenan, T. A. (1983). Recognition of vocal dimensions of emotion: A three-nation study to identify universal characteristics. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 14, 387–406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Van Hooff (1973). A structural analysis of the social behaivour of a semi-captive group of chimpanzees. In M. von Cranach & I. Vine (Eds.), Social Communication and Movement. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
  78. Velle, W. (1992). Sex differences in sensory functions. In J. M. G. van der Dennen (Ed.), The nature of the sexes: The sociobiology of sex differences and the ‘battle of the sexes’ (pp. 29–54). Groningen: Origin Press.Google Scholar
  79. Wallbott, H. G., & Scherer, K. (1986). How universal and specific is emotional experience? Evidence from 27 countries in five continents. Social Science Information, 25, 763–795.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Weisfeld, G. E. (1972). Violations of social norms as inducers of aggression. International Journal of Group Tensions, 2, 53–70.Google Scholar
  81. Weisfeld, G. E. (1980). Social dominance and human motivation. In D. R. Omark, F. F. Strayer & D. G. Freedman (Eds.), Dominance Relations: An ethological view of human conflict and social interaction (pp. 273–286). New York: Garland.Google Scholar
  82. Weisfeld, G. E. (1993). The adaptive value of humor and laughter. Ethology and Sociobiology, 14, 141–169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Weisfeld, G. E. (in press). Discrete emotions theory with specific reference to pride and shame. In N. L. Segal, G. E. Weisfeld & C. C. Weisfeld (Eds.), Genetic, Ethological and Evolutionary Perspectives on Human Development: Essays in honor of Dr. Daniel G. Freedman. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
  84. Weisfeld, G.E., & Billings, R. L. (1988). Observations on adolescence. In K. MacDonald (Ed.), Sociobiological Perspectives in Human Development, pp. 207–233. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  85. Weisfeld, G. E., Russell, R. J. H., Weisfeld, C. C., & Wells, P. A. (1992). Correlates of satisfaction in British marriage. Ethology and Sociobiology, 13, 125–145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. Wolff, P. H. (1969). The natural history of crying and other vocalizations in early infancy. In B. M. Foss (Ed.), Determinants of Infant Behavior (Vol. 4). London: Methuen.Google Scholar
  87. Zivin, G. (1977). Facial gestures predict preschoolers’ encounter outcomes. Social Science Information, 16, 715–730.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Kluwer Academic Publishers 1996

Authors and Affiliations

  • Glenn Weisfeld
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyWayne State UniversityDetroit

Personalised recommendations