Advertisement

The Dynamic Manifestation of Cognitive Structures in the Cerebral Cortex

  • Steven L. Bressler

Abstract

Cognitive structures are organized systems of information that embody the knowledge used to construct an individual’s reality. Having phylogenetic and ontogenetic determinants, they reside latently in the connectional organization of the cerebral cortex, both within and between areas. Different cortical areas, containing separate classes of knowledge in their local associative memories, operate in conjunction with one another to instantiate cognitive structures in perceptuo-motor behavior. As multiple areas recursively interact in largescale networks, their mutual constraint leads to the emergence of coordinated large-scale activity patterns. These patterns constitute a consistent construction of reality that fits the constraints imposed by the structures of the internal and external environments. This construction is the dynamic manifestation of cognitive structures in the cerebral cortex.

Keywords

Cerebral Cortex Cortical Area Local Network Cognitive Structure Connectional Organization 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Barrie, J. M., Freeman, W. J. & Lenhart, M. D. (1996) Spatiotemporal analysis of prepyriform, visual, auditory, and somesthetic surface EEGs in trained rabbits. Journal of Neurophysiology 76: 520–539.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Bressler, S. L. (1994) Dynamic self-organization in the brain as observed by transient cortical coherence. In: Pribram, K. (ed.) Origins: brain and self-organization. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  3. Bressler, S. L. (1995) Large-scale cortical networks and cognition. Brain Research Reviews 20: 288–304.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bressler, S. L. (1996a) Interareal synchronization in the visual cortex. Behavioural Brain Research 76: 37–49.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bressler, S.L. (1996b) Large-scale integration of cortical information processing. In: Pastor, M.A. & Artieda J. (eds.) Time, internal clocks, and movement. Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  6. Bressler, S. L. (1999) Interareal pattern conjunction in the cerebral cortex. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, submitted.Google Scholar
  7. Bressler, S. L., Coppola, R. & Nakamura, R. (1993) Episodic multiregional cortical coherence at multiple frequencies during visual task performance. Nature 366: 153–156.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Callaway, E. M. & Katz, L. C. (1991) Effects of binocular deprivation on the development of clustered horizontal connections in cat striate cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA 88: 745–749.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Churchland, P. S. & Sejnowski, T. J. (1994) The computational brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  10. Damasio, A. R. (1994) Descartes’ error. New York: G. P. Putnam’s Sons.Google Scholar
  11. Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., Tranel, D. & Brandt, J. P. (1990) Neural regionalization of knowledge access: preliminary evidence. Cold Spring Harbor Symposium on Quantitative Biology 50: 1039–1047.Google Scholar
  12. Deese, J. (1970) Psycholinguistics. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.Google Scholar
  13. Edelman, G. (1989) The remembered present: a biological theory of consciousness. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  14. Felleman, D. & Van Essen, D. (1991) Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. Cerebral Cortex 1: 1–47.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Foerster, H. von (1984) On constructing a reality. In: Watzlawick, P. (ed.) The invented reality. New York: Norton & Company.Google Scholar
  16. Fuster, J. M. (1989) The prefrontal cortex: Anatomy, physiology, and neuropsychology of the frontal lobe (2nd ed.). New York: Raven Press.Google Scholar
  17. Gevins, A. S., Morgan, N., Bressler, S., Cutillo, B., White, R., Illes, J., Greer, D., Doyle, J. & Zeitlin, G. (1987) Human neuroelectric patterns predict performance accuracy. Science 235: 580–585.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Glasersfeld, E. von (1995) Radical constructivism: a way of knowing and learning. London: Falmer Press.Google Scholar
  19. Hebb, D. O. (1949) The organization of behavior. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  20. Houk, J. C. Wise, S. P. (1995) Distributed modular architectures linking basal ganglia, cerebellum, and cerebral cortex: Their role in planning and controlling action. Cerebral Cortex 5: 95–110.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Humphreys, G. W. & Riddoch, M. J. (1987) On telling your fruit from your vegetables: a consideration of category-specific deficits after brain damage. Trends in Neurosciences 10: 145–148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Jackendoff, R. (1993) Languages of the mind: Essays on mental representation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  23. Jackendoff, R. (1994) Consciousness and the computational mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  24. Kaas, J. H. (1995) The evolution of isocortex. Brain, Behavior and Evolution 46: 187–196.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Martin, A., Haxby, J. V., Lalonde, F. M., Wiggs, C. L. & Ungerleider, L. G. (1995) Discrete cortical regions associated with knowledge of color and knowledge of action. Science 21Q:102–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. McClelland, J. L. & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981) An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 1. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review 86: 287–330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Mesulam, M. (1994) Neurocognitive networks and selectively distributed processing. Revue Neurologique (Paris) 150: 564–569.Google Scholar
  28. Mumford, D. (1994) Neuronal architectures for pattern-theoretic problems. In: Koch, C. & Davis, J. L. (eds.) Large-scale neuronal theories of the brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  29. Quartz, S. & Sejnowski, T. J. (1997) The neural basis of cognitive development: A constructivist manifesto. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 20: 537–596.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Rauschecker, J. P. (1995) Reverberations of Hebbian thinking. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18: 642–643.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Rescher, N. (1979) Cognitive Systematization. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
  32. Riedl, R. (1984) The consequences of causal thinking. In: Watzlawick, P. (ed.) The invented reality. New York: Norton & Company.Google Scholar
  33. Rumelhart, D. E. & McClelland, J. L. (1986) PDP models and general issues in cognitive science. In: Rumelhart, D., McClelland, J. & The PDP Research Group (eds.) Parallel distributed processing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  34. Singer, W. & Gray, C. M. (1995) Visual feature integration and the temporal correlation hypothesis. Annual Review of Neuroscience 18: 555–586.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Tononi, G., Sporns, O. & Edelman, G. M. (1994) A measure for brain complexity: Relating functional segregation and integration in the nervous system. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA 91: 5033–5037.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Tononi, G., Sporns, O. & Edelman, G. M. (1996) A complexity measure for selective matching of signals by the brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science USA 93: 3422–3427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers 1999

Authors and Affiliations

  • Steven L. Bressler
    • 1
  1. 1.Center for Complex Systems and Brain SciencesFlorida Atlantic UniversityBoca RatonUSA

Personalised recommendations