Skip to main content

Grasping the Dynamic Nature of Intersubjectivity

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Book cover Dynamic Process Methodology in the Social and Developmental Sciences

Abstract

The acknowledgement of the dynamicity of psychological phenomena ®has been progressively gaining acceptance in various branches of psychology. In some of these areas (first of all the neurosciences, psycholinguistic, but also cognitive psychology, and social psychology) the theory has greatly benefited from the adoption of conceptual models and methods of investigation provided by the Dynamic Systems theory (inter alia, Salvatore, Tebaldi, & Potì, 2008). However, in other fields of psychology, authors refer to the dynamic systems in metaphorical terms, using it as a striking image to describe the irreversibility and intrinsic creativity/autonomy of the psychological phenomena under investigation. As a result of this rhetorical strategy, in various areas related to the study of intersubjectivity (work psychology, clinical, and psychodynamic psychology as well as cultural psychology and at least partially developmental psychology) there is an evident gap between the conceptualisation of the phenomena as dynamic and the empirical investigation of it as a “static” process (Lauro-Grotto, Salvatore, Gennaro, & Gelo, 2009—Chapter 1 in this book).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    The assumption of the singleness of the population is more than an implicit assumption grounding the inferential statistic: it is an explicit central concept of it, directly expressed by the null hypothesis that the study has to decide whether or not to accept. In fact, the null hypothesis statement that the two (sets of) observations compared are equivalent corresponds to the claim that they belong to the same population—since their difference is due to the casual variability within the population. On the other hand, the alternative hypothesis statement of a significant difference between the observations compared corresponds to the claim that they belong to different populations—that is the population not affected by the effect of the independent variable and the population affected by it.

References

  • Bakeman, R., & Gottman, J. M. (1997). Observing interaction. An introduction to sequential analysis (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambribge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Barkham, M., Stiles, W. B., & Shapiro, D. A. (1993). The shape of change in psychotherapy: Longitudinal assessment of personal problems. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 667–677.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Borckardt, J. J., et al. (2008). Clinical practice as natural laboratory for psychotherapy research. A guide to case based time-series analysis. American Psychologist, 63(2), 77–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gennaro, A., Salvatore, S., Lis, A., & Salcuni, S. (2008, June 18–22). Looking at the psychotherapy process as an intersubjective dynamic of sensemaking. Paper presented at the 39th Annual Meeting of the Society of Psychotherapy Research Barcelona, Spain.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grassi, R. (2008). Validazione di un modello di analisi delle narrazioni attraverso uno studio idiografico. L’Analisi Frastica Multidimensionale (AFM) [Validation of a model of narrative analysis. An idiographic study with the Multidimensional Utterance Analysis]. Ph.D dissertation, University of Salento—Lecce.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herzberg, F., Mausner, B., & Snyderman, B. B. (1959). The motivation to work (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, I. Z. (1998). Ritual and spontaneity in the psychoanalytic process. Hillsdale, NJ: The Analytic Press Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krause, M. S., Howard, K. I., & Lutz, W. (1998). Exploring individual change. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 838–845.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lambert, M. J. (Ed.). (2004). Bergin and Garfield’s handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (5th ed.). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lancia, F. (2002). The logic of a textscope. Retrieved August 18, 2007, from http://www.tlab.it/en/bibliography.php

  • Lancia, F. (2010 forthcoming). Word co-occurrence and similarity in meaning. In S. Salvatore & J. Valsiner (Eds.), Mind as infinite dimensionality. Roma: Firera Publishing Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langs, R. (1974). The technique of psychoanalytic psychotherapy. New York: Jason Aronson, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laurenceau, J. P., Hayes, A. M., & Feldman, G. C. (2007). Statistical and methodological issues in the study of change in psychotherapy. Clinical Psychology Review, 27, 715–723.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lauro-Grotto, R. P., Salvatore, S., Gennaro, A., & Gelo O. (2009). The unbearable dynamicity of psychological processes: Highlights of the psychodynamic theories. In J. Valsiner, P. Molenaar, M. Lyra, & N. Chaudhary (Eds.), Dynamics process methodology in the social and developmental sciences (pp. 1–30). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mahoney, M. J. (1991). Human change processes. The scientific foundations of psychotherapy. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maruyama, M. (1999). Heterogram analysis: Where the assumption of normal distribution is illogical. Human Systems Management, 18, 53–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mergenthaler, E. (1996). Emotion abstraction patterns in verbatim protocols: A new way of describing therapeutic processes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 1306–1318.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Molenaar, P. C. M., & Valsiner, J. (2005). How generalization works through the single case. A simple idiographic process analysis of an individual psychotherapy. International Journal of Idiographic Science. (Reprinted in S. Salvatore et al. (2009). Yearbook of idiographic science 2008. (Vol. 1). Rome: G Fireira Group)

    Google Scholar 

  • Nightingale, D. J., & Cromby, J. (Eds.). (1999). Social constructionist psychology. A critical analysis of theory and practice. Buckingham: Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., & Tannenbaum, T. H. (1957). The measurement of meaning. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salvatore, S., Gelo, O., Gennaro, A., Manzo, S., & Al-Radaideh, A. (in press). Looking at the psychotherapy process as an intersubjective dynamics of meaning making. A case study with Discourse Flow Analysis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salvatore, S., Lis, A., Salcuni, S., Serio, A. V., Gennaro, A., Mossi, P. et al. (2006, September 28–October 1). Confronto tra studio delle difese e della dinamica discorsiva nell’analisi del processo terapeutico. Studio di un caso [Comparison of defense mechanisms and discursive dynamics in the therapeutic process. A case study]. Paper presented at the 6th Annual Meeting of the Italian Chapter of the Society of Psychotherapy Research. Reggio Calabria, Italy.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salvatore, S., Tebaldi, C., & Potì, S. (2008). The discoursive dynamic of sensemaking. In S. Salvatore, J. Valsiner, S. Strout, & J. Clegg (Eds.), Yearbook of idiographic science 2008 (pp. 39–72). Rome: Firera Publishing Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salvatore, S., & Venuleo, C. (2009) The unconscious as source of sense: A psychodynamic approach to meaning. In B. Wagoner (Ed.), Symbolic transformations: The mind in movement through culture and society. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schaie, K. W., Willis, S. L., & Pennak, S. (2005). A historical framework for cohort differences in intelligence. Research in Human Development, 2,43–67.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Semerari, A., Carcione, A., Dimaggio, G., Falcone, M., Nicolo, G., Procacci, M., et al. (2003). Assessing problematic states inside patient’s narratives. The grid of problematic conditions. Psychotherapy Research, 13(3), 337–353.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • TLAB—Tools for text analysis. By T-LAB® di Lancia Franco. Retrieved August 10, 2007, from http://tlab.it

  • Toomela, A. (2008). Variables in psychology: A critique of quantitative psychology. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 42(3), 245–265.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Toomela, A. (2009). How methodology became a toolbox—and how it escapes from that box. In J. Valsiner, P. C. M. Moolenar, M. C. D. P. Lyra, & N. Chaudhary (Eds.), Dynamic process methodology in the social and developmental sciences (pp. 45–66). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tschacher, W., Schiepek, G., & Brummer, E. J. (Eds.). (1992). Self-organization and clinical psychology. Empirical approaches to synergetics in psychology. Berlin: Springer Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J., Salvatore, S., Strout, S., & Clegg, J. (Eds.). (2009). Yearbook of idiographic science 2008 (Vol. 1). Rome: Firera Publishing Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Venuleo, C., & Salvatore, S. (2006). Linguaggi e dispositivi comunicativi. I Faccia a Faccia televisivi [Languages and communication devices. Face to face television debates]. In S. Cristante, & P. Mele (Eds.), Da Vendola a Prodi. I media nelle campagne elettorali 2005–2006 (pp. 151–189). Nardò, LE: Besa Editrice.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgement

We wish to thank the participants in the session of the Clark University’s Kitchen Seminar (10th of September, 2008) devoted to the discussion of a first draft of this text. The discussion that developed there provided meaningful feedback, helping us to bring the implications of our proposal into focus.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Sergio Salvatore .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2009 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Salvatore, S., Lauro-Grotto, R., Gennaro, A., Gelo, O. (2009). Grasping the Dynamic Nature of Intersubjectivity. In: Valsiner, J., Molenaar, P., Lyra, M., Chaudhary, N. (eds) Dynamic Process Methodology in the Social and Developmental Sciences. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-95922-1_8

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics