Skip to main content

How Methodology Became a Toolbox—And How it Escapes from that Box

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Book cover Dynamic Process Methodology in the Social and Developmental Sciences

Abstract

The understanding that research methodology comprises an essential part of scientific theories about phenomena that are studied is not always brought into the center of theoretical reasoning. The inevitable connection of methodology and statements about the nature of the phenomenon studied becomes obvious when we ask for the proofs of theoretical statements about the phenomenon that a theory should explain. The only scientific way to give the proofs for a theory requires description of the research methodology—who was studied with what assessment methods and which was the exact procedure of data collection. If the analysis of the research methodology reveals questionable procedures or implicit restrictive theoretically not justified assumptions, all theory based on such a methodology must be questioned as well.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Allik, J., & McCrae, R. R. (2002). A five-factor theory perspective. In R. R. McCrae & J. Allik (Eds.), The five-factor model of personality across cultures (pp. 303–322). New York: Kluwer Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allport, G. W. (1937). Personality. A psychological interpretation. New York: Henry Holt and Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anokhin, P. K. (1975). Ocherki po fiziologii funktsional’nykh sistem. Moscow: Medicina.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anokhin, P. K. (1978a). Izbrannyje trudy. Filosofskije aspekty teorii funktsional’noi sistemy. In F. K. Konstantinov, B. F. Lomov, & B. V. Shvyrkov (Eds.). Moscow: Nauka.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anokhin, P. K. (1978b). Metodologicheskii analiz uzlovykh problem uslovnogo refleksa. (Methodological analysis of central problems of the conditioned reflex). In F. V. Konstantinov, B. F. Lomov, & V. B. Schvyrkov (Eds.), P. K. Anokhin. Izbrannyje trudy. Filosofskije aspekty teorii funktsional’noi sistemy (pp. 152–206). Moscow: Nauka. (In Russian. Original work published 1963)

    Google Scholar 

  • Aristotle (1941). Metaphysics. In R. McKeon (Ed.), The basic works of Aristotle (pp. 681–926). New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, R. L., Atkinson, R. C., Smith, E. E., Bem, D. J., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1993). Introduction to psychology (11th ed.). Fort Worth: Harcourt Brace College Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bem, S., & Looren de Jong, H. (1997). Theoretical issues in psychology. London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein, D. A., Roy, E. J., Srull, T. K., & Wickens, C. D. (1988). Psychology. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borsboom, D., Mellenbergh, G. J., & van Heerden, J. (2004). The concept of validity. Psychological Review, 111, 1061–1071.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlile, W. (1895a). Reality and causation. Part I. Mind, 4, 82–91.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlile, W. (1895b). Reality and causation. Part II. Mind, 4, 213–224.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, N. R., Buskist, W., Martin, G. N., Hogg, M., & Abrams, D. (1997). Psychology. The science of behaviour. European adaptation. Harlow, England: Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Case, R. (1992). The mind’s staircase. Exploring the conceptual underpinnings of children’s thought and knowledge. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Descartes, R. (1985a). Meditations on first philosophy. In J. Cottingham, R. Stoothoff, & D. Murdoch (Trans.), The philosophical writings of Descartes (Vol. 2, pp. 3–62). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. (Original work published 1641)

    Google Scholar 

  • Descartes, R. (1985b). Objections and replies. In J. Cottingham, R. Stoothoff, & D. Murdoch (Trans.). The philosophical writings of Descartes (Vol. 2, pp. 66–397). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. (Original work published 1641)

    Google Scholar 

  • Descartes, R. (1985c). The passions of the soul. In J. Cottingham, R. Stoothoff, & D. Murdoch (Trans.). The philosophical writings of Descartes (Vol. 1, pp. 326–404). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. (Original work published 1649)

    Google Scholar 

  • Descartes, R. (1985d). Principles of philosophy. In J. Cottingham, R. Stoothoff, & D. Murdoch (Trans.). The philosophical writings of Descartes (Vol. 1, pp. 179–291). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. (Original work published 1644)

    Google Scholar 

  • Descartes, R. (1985e). Rules for the direction of the mind. In J. Cottingham, R. Stoothoff, & D. Murdoch (Trans.). The philosophical writings of Descartes (Vol. 1, pp. 9–78). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. (Composed in 1628)

    Google Scholar 

  • Endler, N. S., & Speer, R. L. (1998). Personality psychology: Research trends for 1993–1995. Journal of Personality, 66, 622–669.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, R. S. (1993). Understanding psychology. (3rd ed., International edition). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, K. W., & Bidell, T. R. (1998). Dynamic development of psychological structures in action and thought. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology. Vol. 1: Theoretical models of human development (5th ed., pp. 467–561). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gleitman, H., Fridlund, A. J., & Reisberg, D. (1999). Psychology (5th ed.). New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grusec, J. E., Lockhart, R. S., & Walters, G. C. (1990). Foundations of psychology. Toronto: Copp Clark Pitman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hegel, G. W. H. (1969). Science of logic. In H. D. Lewis (Ed.), Hegel’s science of logic. New York: Humanity Books. (Original work published 1831)

    Google Scholar 

  • Hume, D. (1999). An enquiry concerning human understanding. In T. L. Beauchamp (Ed.), David Hume. An enquiry concerning human understanding. Oxford: Oxford University Press. (Original work published 1748)

    Google Scholar 

  • Hume, D. (2000). A treatise of human nature. In D. F. Norton & M. J. Norton (Eds.), David Hume. A treatise of human nature. Oxford: Oxford University Press. (Original work published 1739–1740)

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1950). The principles of psychology. Authorized edition in two volumes. New York: Dover Publications. (Original work published 1890)

    Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, A. (1998). The g factor. The science of mental ability. Westport: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirkpatrick, E. A. (1909). Genetic psychology. An introduction to an objective and genetic view of intelligence. New York: MacMillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kleinginna, P. R., & Kleinginna, A. M. (1981a). A categorized list of emotion definitions, with suggestions for a consensual definition. Motivation and Emotion, 5, 345–379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kleinginna, P. R., & Kleinginna, A. M. (1981b). A categorized list of motivation definitions, with a suggestion for a consensual definition. Motivation and Emotion, 5, 263–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koffka, K. (1935). Principles of gestalt psychology. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Köhler, W. (1940). Dynamics in psychology. New York: Washington Square Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Köhler, W. (1959). Gestalt psychology. An introduction to new concepts in modern psychology. New York: Mentor Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kroeber, A. L., & Kluckhohn, C. (1952). Culture: A critical review of concepts and definitions. Papers. Peabody museum of archaeology and ethnology (Vol. 47, pp. 1–223). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. S. (1970). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Külpe, O. (1909). Outlines of psychology. London: Swan Sonnenschein.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ladd, G. T. (1894). Psychology descriptive and explanatory. New York: Charles Scribner’s Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh. The embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewin, K. (1935). A dynamic theory of personality. Selected papers. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R. (1969). Vyshije korkovyje funktsii tsheloveka i ikh narushenija pri lokal’nykh porazenijakh mozga. [Higher cortical functions in man and their disturbances in local brain lesions]. Moscow: Izdatel’stvo Moskovskogo Universiteta.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R. (1973). Osnovy neiropsikhologii. Moscow: Izdatel’stvo MGU.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R. (1979). Jazyk i soznanije. Moscow: Izdatel’stvo Moskovskogo Universiteta.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mackintosh, N. J. (1998). IQ and human intelligence. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mallon, S. D., Kingsley, D., Affleck, G., & Tennen, H. (1998). Methodological trends in journal of personality: 1970–1995. Journal of Personality, 66, 672–685.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marsella, A. J., Dubanoski, J., Hamada, W. C., & Morse, H. (2000). The measurement of personality across cultures. American Behavioral Scientist, 44, 41–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1996). Toward a new generation of personality theories: Theoretical contexts for the five-factor model. In J. S. Wiggins (Ed.), The five-factor model of personality. (pp. 51–87). New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T. (1999). A five-factor theory of personality. In A. Lawrence & O. P. J. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 139–153). New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Molenaar, P. C. M. (2004). A manifesto on psychology as idiographic science: Bringing the person back into scientific psychology, this time forever. Measurement, 2, 201–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Molenaar, P. C. M., & Valsiner, J. (2005). How generalization works through the single case: A simple idiographic process analysis of an individual psychotherapy. International Journal of Idiographic Science, Article. 1. Retrieved October 25, 2005, from http://www.valsiner.com/articles/molenvals.htm.

  • Myers, D. G. (1995). Psychology (4th ed.). New York: Worth Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pearl, J. (2000). Causality. Models, reasoning, and inference. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poortinga, Y. H., & van Hemert, D. A. (2001). Personality and culture: Demarcating between the common and the unique. Journal of Personality, 69, 1033–1060.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roediger, H. L., Capaldi, E. D., Paris, S. G., Polivy, J., & Herman, C. P. (1996). Psychology (4th ed.). Minneapolis: West Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sloman, S. (2005). Causal models. How people think about the world and its alternatives. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. E., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Fredrickson, B. L., Loftus, G. R., Bem, D. J., & Maren, S. (2003). Atkinson and Hilgard’s introduction to psychology (14th ed.) Belmont, CA: Wadsworth/Thomson Learning.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, L. B., & Thelen, E. (1993). A dynamic systems approach to development. Applications. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spirtes, P., Glymour, C., & Scheines, R. (2000). Causation, prediction, and search. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sully, J. (1892). The human mind. A text-book of psychology. In two volumes (Vol. 1). London: Longmans, Green and Co.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E. (1995). Motor development. A new synthesis. American Psychologist, 50, 79–95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (1994). A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Titchener, E. B. (1898). Postulates of structural psychology. Philosophical Review, 7, 449–465.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Titchener, E. B. (1899). Structural and functional psychology. Philosophical Review, 8, 290–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toomela, A. (1996). How culture transforms mind: A process of internalization. Culture and Psychology, 2, 285–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toomela, A. (2000a). Activity theory is a dead end for cultural-historical psychology. Culture and Psychology, 6, 353–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toomela, A. (2000b). Cultural-historical psychology: Three levels of analysis. Dissertationes Psychologicae Universitatis Tartuensis, 7, University of Tartu, Tartu.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toomela, A. (2000c). Stages of mental development: Where to look? Trames: Journal of the Humanities and Social Sciences, 4, 21–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toomela, A. (2003a). Culture as a semiosphere: On the role of culture in the culture-individual relationship. In I. E. Josephs (Ed.), Dialogicality in development (pp. 129–163). Westport, CT: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toomela, A. (2003b). Development of symbol meaning and the emergence of the semiotically mediated mind. In A. Toomela (Ed.), Cultural guidance in the development of the human mind. (pp. 163–209). Westport, CT: Ablex Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toomela, A. (2003c). Relationships between personality structure, structure of word meaning, and cognitive ability: A study of cultural mechanisms of personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 723–735.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toomela, A. (2005). Decision-making with incomplete information: Systemic and non-systemic ways of thinking in medicine. In R. Bibace, J. Laird, & J. Valsiner (Eds.), Science and medicine in dialogue: Thinking through particulars and universals (pp. 231–241). Westport, CT: Greenwood/Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toomela, A. (2007a). Culture of science: Strange history of the methodological thinking in psychology. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 41, 6–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toomela, A. (2007b). History of methodology in psychology: Starting point, not the goal. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 41, 75–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toomela, A. (2007c). Unifying psychology: Absolutely necessary, not only useful. In A. V. B. Bastos & N. M. D. Rocha (Eds.), Psicologia: Novas direçoes no dialogo com outros campos de saber. (pp. 449–464). São Paulo: Casa do Psicologo.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toomela, A. (2008a). Activity theory is a dead end for methodological thinking in cultural psychology too. Culture and Psychology, 14, 289–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toomela, A. (2008b). Variables in psychology: A critique of quantitative psychology. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 42, 245–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toomela, A. (2008c). Vygotskian cultural-historical and sociocultural approaches represent two levels of analysis: Complementarity instead of opposition. Culture and Psychology, 14, 57–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toomela, A. (2009a). Kurt Lewin’s contribution to the methodology of psychology: From past to future skipping the present. In J. Clegg (Ed.), The observation of human systems. Lessons from the history of anti-reductionistic empirical psychology (pp. 101–116). Edison, NJ: Transaction Publisher.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toomela, A. (2009b). Methodology of idiographic science: Limits of single-case studies and the role of typology. In S. Salvatore, et al. (Eds.), YIS: Yearbook of Idiographic Science, 2008. Rome: Giorgio Fireira.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toomela, A. (in press). Poverty of modern mainstream psychology in autobiography. Reflections on the A History of Psychology in Autobiography, (Vol. 9) (G. Lindzey & W. M. Runyan (Eds.) (2007). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association). Culture and Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (2003). Assumptions and knowledge constructions: what can science learn from primate languages and cultures? In A. Toomela (Ed.), Cultural guidance in the development of the human mind (pp. 39–59). Westport, CT: Ablex Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Bertalanffy, L. (1968). General systems theory. Foundations, development, applications. New York: George Braziller.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1982a). Istoricheski smysl psikhologicheskogo krizisa. In A. R. Luria & M. G. Jaroshevskii (Eds.), L. S. Vygotsky. Sobranije sochinenii. Tom 1. Voprosy teorii i istorii psikhologii (pp. 291–436). Moscow: Pedagogika. (Originally written in 1927)

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1982b). Psikhologija i uchenije o lokalizacii psikhicheskih funktcii. In A. R. Luria & M. G. Jaroshevskii (Eds.), L. S. Vygotsky. Sobranije sochinenii. Tom 1. Voprosy teorii i istorii psikhologii. (pp. 168–174). Moscow: Pedagogika. (Originally written in 1934)

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1994). The problem of the cultural development of the child. In R. van der Veer & J. Valsiner (Eds.), The Vygotsky reader. (pp. 57–72). Oxford: Blackwell. (Original work published 1929)

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1996). Myshlenije i rech. (Thinking and speech). Moscow: Labirint. (Original work published 1934)

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S., & Luria, A. (1994). Tool and symbol in child development. In R. van der Veer & J. Valsiner (Eds.), The Vygotsky reader (pp. 99–174). Oxford, UK: Blackwell. (Originally written in 1930)

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, G. (1934). Psychology in Germany and Austria. Psychological Bulletin, 31, 755–776.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Werner, H. (1948). Comparative psychology of mental development. New York: International Universities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wertheimer, M. (1925). Über Gestaltheorie. Erlangen: Weltkreis-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wundt, W. (1897). Outlines of psychology. Leipzig: Wilhelm Engelman.

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Aaro Toomela .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2009 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Toomela, A. (2009). How Methodology Became a Toolbox—And How it Escapes from that Box. In: Valsiner, J., Molenaar, P., Lyra, M., Chaudhary, N. (eds) Dynamic Process Methodology in the Social and Developmental Sciences. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-95922-1_3

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics