Advertisement

Social Dynamics in Complex Family Contexts and its Study

  • Nandita Chaudhary
Chapter

Abstract

The prevailing patterns of social relationships within a culture are important to consider during research. Since culture is considered as constitutive for individual psyche, the manner in which research tasks are set-up also needs to appraisal in the context of prevailing social networks in any given society. This paper deals with examples of research studies among Asian Indians to describe encounters with participants during research which highlight the importance of considering and incorporating cultural patterns of interaction. Some of the simple ways of engaging with people and ideas can be achieved by keeping intuitive reactions intact and working with participants as partners in and not objects of research. Creating and following mythical rituals (like random sampling, or standardised testing) over and above the personal reality that is critical to meaningful social experiences, provides research with a sacred status; but it also distances science from everyday life, consequently defeating the very purpose for which the efforts have been undertaken.

Keywords

Familiar Object Person Approach Individual Opinion Indian Family Ground Reality 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Notes

Acknowledgements

I would like to acknowledge the contributions made by Prof. Heidi Keller (University of Osnabrueck, Germany) and Prof. Jaan Valsiner (Clark University Worcester, USA) for their perceptive and pertinent reactions to earlier versions of this chapter. These comments helped me to become self-conscious of my occasional imprudence in discussing issues of theory and method.

References

  1. Anandalakshmy, A., Sharma, N., & Chaudhary, N. (Eds.). (2008). Constructing research methods: Insights from the field. New Delhi: Sage.Google Scholar
  2. Beckstead, Z., & Valsiner, J. (2008, August 28). The dialogical self of the researcher-as-person who enters into a dialogue with the dialogical self of the research participant-as-person. Paper presented at the 5th International Conference on Dialogical Self, Symposium titled, Dialogical selves in developmental research practice, Queens College, Cambridge, UK.Google Scholar
  3. Berreby, D. (2005). Us and them: Understanding our tribal minds. New York: Little, Brown and Company.Google Scholar
  4. Bhargava P., & Chaudhary, N. (2006, July 11–15). Cultural reconstruction of methods in Indian families. Paper presented at the International conference of the IACCP, Spetses, Greece.Google Scholar
  5. Bowlby, J. (1988/1997). A secure base: Clinical applications of attachment theory. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  6. Caudill, W., & Plath, D. W. (1966). Who sleeps by whom? Parent-child involvement in urban Japanese families. Psychiatry, 29(2), 344–366.Google Scholar
  7. Chaudhary, N. (2004). Listening to culture: Constructing reality from everyday talk. New Delhi: Sage.Google Scholar
  8. Chaudhary, N. (2005). Researching communities: Travails of working with Indian communities. Cross-cultural Psychology Bulletin. International Association for Cross-cultural Psychology, 43(4), 5–13.Google Scholar
  9. Chaudhary, N., & Bhargava, P. (2006). Mamta: The transformation of meaning in everyday usage. Contributions to Indian Sociology, 40(3), 343–375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Cole, M. (1996). Cultural psychology: A once and future discipline. Cambridge, MA: Belknap/Harvard.Google Scholar
  11. D’Andrade, R. G. (1984). Cultural meaning systems. In R. A. Shweder & R. A. LeVine (Eds.), Culture theory: Essays on mind, self and emotion (pp 88–122). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  12. Danziger, K. (1997). Naming the mind: How psychology found its language. London: Sage.Google Scholar
  13. Derrida, J., & Spivak, G. C. (1976). Grammatology (G. C. Spivak, Trans.). Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
  14. Edwards, C. P. (2005). Children’s play in cross-cultural perspective: A new look at the six cultures study. Faculty publications, Department of Family and Consumer Sciences, University of Nebraska.Google Scholar
  15. Greenfield, P. M., Keller, H., Fulgini, A., & Maynard, A. (2003). Cultural pathways through universal development. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 461–490.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Gould, S. J. (2003). The fox, the hedgehog and the magister’s pox. New York: Three rivers press.Google Scholar
  17. Gunther, I. A. (1998). Contacting subjects: The untold story. Culture and Psychology, 4(1), 65–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Gupta, D., Shukla, S., & Chaudhary, N. (2008, July). Intra-cultural variation in mirror-self-recognition among young Indian children. Paper presented at International conference of the International Association for Cross-cultural Psychology, Bremen, Germany.Google Scholar
  19. Hermans, H. H. (2001). The dialogical self: Towards a theory of cultural and personal positioning. Culture and Psychology, 7(3), 243–281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Jahoda, G. (2002). The shifting sands of ‘culture’. In P. Boski, F. J. R. Van der Vijver, & A. M. Chodynicka (Eds.), New directions in cross-cultural psychology (pp. 91–106). Warsaw: Polish Psychological Association.Google Scholar
  21. Kagitcibasi, C. (1996). The autonomous—relational self: A new synthesis. European Psychologist, 1(3), 180–186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Kakar, S. (1981). The inner world: A psycho-analytic study of childhood and society in India. Delhi, India: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  23. Kaura, I. (2008). Making sense of what they say. In S. Anandalakshmy, N. Chaudhary, & N. Sharma (Eds.), Researching families and children: Culturally appropriate methods (pp. 156–163). New Delhi: Sage.Google Scholar
  24. Keller, H. (2007). Cultures of infancy. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  25. Levinas, E. (1969). Totality and infinity: An essay on exteriority (A. Lingis, Trans.). Pittsburgh: Duquesne University Press.Google Scholar
  26. Luria, A. R. (1976). Cognitive development: Its cultural and social foundations. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  27. Marriott, M. (1989). Constructing an Indian ethnosociology. In M. Marriott (Ed.), India through Hindu categories. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  28. Mead, G. H. (2001). The philosophies of Royce, James and Dewey in their American setting. Culture and Psychology, 7(1), 49–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Mistry, J., & Saraswathi, T. S. (2003). Culture and child development. In I. Siegel (Series Ed.) & R. L. Lerner, A. Easterbrooks, & J. Mistry (Vol. Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Vol.6. Developmental Psychology. New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
  30. Moos, R. H., & Moos, B. S. (1994). Family environment scale manual (3rd ed). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
  31. Rao, K. R. (2008). Prologue: Introducing Indian psychology. In K. R. Rao, A. C. Paranjpe, & A. K. Dalal (Eds.), Handbook of Indian psychology (pp. 1–19). New Delhi: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Ratner, C. (2008). Cultural psychology and qualitative methodology. Scientific and political considerations. Culture and Psychology, 14(3), 259–288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Reddy, V. (2008). How infants know minds. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  34. Roland, A. (1988). In search of self in India and Japan: Towards a cross-cultural psychology. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  35. Sinha, D. (2002) Culture and psychology: Perspective of Cross-cultural Psychology. Psychology and Developing Societies, 14, 11–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Scribner, S. (1976). Situating the experiment in cross-cultural research. In K. E. Reigel & J. A. Meancham (Eds.), The developing individual in a changing world (Vol. 1, pp. 310–321). Montana: Hague.Google Scholar
  37. Shweder, R. A. (1984). Anthropology’s romantic rebellion against enlightenment, or there’s more to thinking than reason and evidence. In R. A. Shweder & R. A. LeVine (Eds.), Culture theory: Essays on mind, self and emotion (pp. 27–66). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  38. Shweder, R. A., Jensen, L. A., & Goldstein, W. M. (1995). Who sleeps by whom revisited: A method for extracting the moral goods implicit in practice. In J. J. Goodnow, P. J. Miller, & F. Kessel (Eds.), Cultural practices as contexts for development. New directions for child development, 76. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass Inc.Google Scholar
  39. Sindhu, N. (1978). Cognitive development in infancy: Maternal and nutritional correlates. Unpublished masters dissertation. Department of Child Development, Lady Irwin College. University of Delhi—New Delhi.Google Scholar
  40. Spivak, G. C. (2004). Death of a discipline. Calcutta: Seagull Books.Google Scholar
  41. Tomar, R. (2009). Pre-school children’s conformity and its relation to selected factors: Authority, reciprocity, reasoning and majority opinion. Ongoing masters dissertation. Department of Human Development and Childhood Studies. Lady Irwin College. University of Delhi—New Delhi.Google Scholar
  42. Tuli, M., & Chaudhary, N. (2008). Cultural networks, social research and contact sampling. In S. Anandalakshmy, N. Chaudhary, & N. Sharma (Eds.), Researching families and children: Culturally appropriate methods (pp. 53–66). New Delhi: Sage.Google Scholar
  43. Valsiner, J., & Rudolph, L. (2008, July 21). Who will survive? Psychology that replaces quantification with qualitative mathematics. Paper presented at the symposium entitled ‘Why psychology moves towards qualitative epistemological foundations?’ International Congress of Psychology, Berlin, Germany.Google Scholar
  44. Valsiner, J., & Sato, T. (2006). Historically structured sampling (HSS): How can Psychology’s methodology become tuned into the reality and the historical nature of cultural psychology. In J. Straub, D. Weidemenn, C. Koelbl, & B. Zeilke (Eds.), Pursuit of meaning: Advances in cultural and cross-cultural Psychology (pp. 215–251). Bielefeld: Verlag.Google Scholar
  45. Weisner, T. S. (2005). Discovering successful pathways in children’s development. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  46. Wetherell, M., & Potter, J. (1992). Mapping the language of racism: Discourse and the legitimisation of exploitation. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
  47. Yoshikawa, H., Weisner, T. S., Kalil, A., & Way, N. (2008). Mixing qualitative and quantitative research in developmental science: Uses and methodological choices. Developmental Psychology, 44(2), 344–354.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Human Development and Childhood Studies, Lady Irwin CollegeUniversity of DelhiNew DelhiIndia

Personalised recommendations