Advertisement

Multilevel Simultaneous Component Analysis for Studying Intra-Individual Variability and Inter-Individual Differences

  • Marieke E. Timmerman
  • Eva Ceulemans
  • Anna Lichtwarck-Aschoff
  • Kristof Vansteelandt
Chapter

Abstract

In psychology, the distinction between traits and states has become commonplace for a long time: Whereas traits pertain to person features that are relatively stable and consistent, states refer to person features that fluctuate across time, for example as a result of situational influences. For instance, Allport and Odbert (1936) provided an extensive list of trait and state terms to characterize personality and personal behavior. The trait terms describe permanent, consistent dispositions and were considered to ‘symbolize most clearly “real” traits of personality’ (Allport & Odbert, 1936, p. 26). The state terms describe ‘Temporary Moods or Activities’. They were not considered to symbolize personality, and were merely included for the sake of completeness.

Keywords

Physical Activity Negative Affect Eating Disorder Bulimia Nervosa Measurement Occasion 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Abe, J. A., & Izard, C. E. (1999). The developmental functions of emotions: An analysis in terms of differential emotions theory. Cognition & Emotion, 13, 523–549.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Allport, G. W., & Odbert, H. S. (1936). Trait-names. A psycho-lexical study. Psychological monographs, (Vol. 47, Whole No. 211).Google Scholar
  3. Arrindell, W. A., & Ettema, J. H. M. (1986). SCL-90: Handleiding bij een multidimensionale psychopathologie-indicator. Klachtenlijst SCL-90 (SCL-90). Lisse: Swets Test Services.Google Scholar
  4. Beumont, P. J. V., Arthur, B., Russell, J. D., & Touyz, S. W. (1994). Excessive physical activity in dieting disorder patients: Proposals for a supervised exercise program. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 15, 21–36.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bijleveld, C. C. J. H., & Bijleveld, F. D. (1997). A comparison of the applicability of two approaches to linear dynamic systems analysis for N subjects. Kwantitatieve Methoden, 55, 37–56.Google Scholar
  6. Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1992). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. Sociological Methods and Research, 21, 230–258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Cattell, R. B. (1952). The three basic factor-analytic research designs-their interrelations and derivatives. Psychological Bulletin, 49, 499–520.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Cattell, R. B. (1966). The scree test for the number of factors. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 1, 245–276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Davis, C. (1997). Eating disorders and hyperactivity: A psychobiological perspective. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 42, 168–175.Google Scholar
  10. Davis, C., Katzman, D. K., & Kirsh, C. (1999). Compulsive physical activity in adolescents with anorexia nervosa: A psychobehavioral spiral of pathology. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 187, 336–342.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Davis, C., Kennedy, S. H., Ravelski, E., & Dionne, M. (1994). The role of physical activity in the development and maintenance of eating disorders. Psychological Medicine, 24, 957–967.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Du Toit, S. H. C., & Du Toit, M. (2008). Multilevel structural equation modeling. In J. De Leeuw & E. Meijer (Eds.), Handbook of multilevel analysis. New York: Springer Verlag.Google Scholar
  13. Flury, B. (1988). Common principal components & related multivariate models. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  14. Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  15. Frijda, N. H. (2001). The self and emotions. In H. A. Bosma & E. S. Kunnen (Eds.), Identity and emotion: Development through self-organization. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  16. Garner, D. M. (1991). Eating disorder inventory-2 manual. Odessa: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
  17. Gottman, J. M., & Levenson, R. W. (1999). Rebound from marital conflict and divorce prediction. Family Process, 38(3), 287–292.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Gower, J. C., & Dijksterhuis, G. B. (2004). Procrustes problems. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Granic, I., Hollenstein, T., Dishion, T. J., & Patterson, G. R. (2003). Longitudinal analysis of flexibility and reorganization in early adolescence: A dynamic systems study of family interactions. Developmental Psychology, 39, 606–617.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Guerrero, L. K., & La Valley, A. G. (2006). Conflict, emotion, and communication. In J. G. Oetzel & S. Ting-Toomey (Eds.), The sage handbook of conflict communication: Integrating theory, research, and practice (pp. 69–96). Thousand Oaks: Sage.Google Scholar
  21. Hamaker, E. L., Nesselroade, J. R., & Molenaar, P. C. M. (2007). The integrated trait-state model. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 295–315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Heatherton, T. F., & Baumeister, R. F. (1991). Binge eating as escape from self awareness. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 86–108.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Hollenstein, T., Granic, I., Stoolmiller, M., & Snyder, J. (2004). Rigidity in parent–child interactions and the development of externalizing and internalizing behavior in early childhood. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 32, 595–607.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Holtkamp, K., Hebebrand, J., & Herpertz-Dahlmann, B. (2004). The contribution of anxiety and food restriction on physical activity levels in acute anorexia nervosa. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 36, 163–171.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Hurlburt, R. T., Lech, B., & Saltman, S. (1984). Random sampling of thought and mood. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 8, 263–275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Jolliffe, I. T. (2002). Principal component analysis. New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  27. Jones, C. J., & Nesselroade, J. R. (1990). Multivariate, replicated, single-subject, repeated measures designs and P-technique factor analysis: A review of intraindividual change studies. Experimental Aging Research, 16, 171–183.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Jöreskog, K. G. (1971). Simultaneous factor analysis in several populations. Psychometrika, 36, 409–426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Kaiser, H. F. (1958). The varimax criterion for analytic rotation in factor analysis. Psychometrika, 23, 187–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2000). Beyond valence: Toward a model of emotion-specific influences on judgment and choice. Cognition and Emotion, 14, 473–493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Lewis, M. D. (1995). Cognition-emotion feedback and the self-organization of developmental paths. Human Development, 38, 71–102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Lewis, M. D. (2000). Emotional self-organization at three time scales. In M. D. Lewis & I. Granic (Eds.), Emotion, development, and self-organization: Dynamic systems approaches to emotional development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Magai, C., & McFadden, S. H. (1995). The role of emotions in social and personality development: Hirstory, theory, and research. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  34. Magai, C., & Nusbaum, B. (1996). Personality change in adulthood. In C. Magai & S. H. McFadden (Eds.), Handbook of emotion, adult development, and aging (pp. 403–420). San Diego: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Muthén, B. O. (1989). Latent variable modelling in heterogeneous populations. Psychometrika, 54, 557–585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Nesselroade, J. R. (2007). Factoring at the Individual level: Some matters for the second century of factor analysis. In R. Cudeck & R. C. MacCallum (Eds.), Factor analysis at 100: Historical developments and future directions (pp. 249–264). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  37. Nesselroade, J. R., & Molenaar, P. C. M. (1999). Pooling lagged covariance structures based on short, multivariate time series for dynamic factor analysis. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Statistical strategies for small sample research (pp. 223–250). Thousand Oaks: Sage.Google Scholar
  38. Reise, S. P., Ventura, J., Nuechterlein, K. H., & Kim, K. H. (2005). An illustration of multilevel factor analysis. Journal of Personality Assessment, 84, 126–136.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Roseman, I. J., & Evdokas, A. (2004). Appraisals cause experienced emotions: Experimental evidence. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 1–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Sanford, K. (2007). Hard and soft emotion during conflict: Investigating married couples and other relationships. Personal Relationships, 14, 65–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Shifren, K., Hooker, K. Wood, P., & Nesselroade, J. R. (1997). Structure and variation of mood in individuals with Parkinson’s disease: A dynamic factor analysis. Psychology and Aging, 12, 328–339.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Solenberger, S. E. (2001). Exercise and eating disorders: A 3-year inpatient hospital record analysis. Eating Behaviors, 2, 151–168.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Spielberger, C. D. (1972). Anxiety: Current trends in theory and research (Vol. 1). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  44. Strayer, J. (2002). The dynamics of emotions and life cycle identity. Identity, 2, 47–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Ten Berge, J. M. F. (1986). Rotation to perfect congruence and the cross-validation of component weights across populations. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 21, 41–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Thome, J., & Espelage, D. L. (2004). Relations among exercise, coping, disordered eating, and psychological health among college students. Eating Behavior, 5, 337–351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Timmerman, M. E. (2006). Multilevel component analysis. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 59, 301–320.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Timmerman, M. E., & Kiers, H. A. L. (2003). Four simultaneous component models of multivariate time series from more than one subject to model intraindividual and interindividual differences. Psychometrika, 86, 105–122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Timmerman, M. E., Kiers, H. A. L., Smilde, A. K., Ceulemans, E., & Stouten, J. (2009). Bootstrap confidence intervals in multilevel simultaneous component analysis. British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology. 62, 299–318.Google Scholar
  50. Tucker, L. R. (1951). A method for synthesis of factor analysis studies (Personnel Research Section Report No. 984). Washington, DC: Department of the Army.Google Scholar
  51. Vansteelandt, K., Rijmen, F., Pieters, G., Probst, M., & Vanderlinden, J. (2007). Drive for thinness, affect regulation and physical activity in eating disorders: A daily life study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 1717–1734.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Vansteelandt, K., Van Mechelen, I., & Nezlek, J. B. (2005). The co-occurrence of emotions in daily life: A multilevel approach. Journal of Research in Personality, 39, 325–335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Van Strien, T. (2002). Eating disorder inventory-II Dutch version (EDI-II-NL). Amsterdam: Swets Test Publishers.Google Scholar
  54. Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1994). The PANAS-X. Manual for the positive and negative affect schedule—Expanded form. Retrieved March 26, 2008, from http://www.psychology.uiowa.edu/Faculty/Watson/PANAS-X.pdf
  55. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief measures of positive an negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Widaman, K. F. (1993). Common factor analysis versus principal component analysis: Differential bias in representing model parameters? Multivariate Behavioral Research, 28, 263–311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Widaman, K. F. (2007). Common factors versus components: Principals and principles, errors and misconceptions. In R. Cudeck & R. C. MacCallum (Eds.), Factor analysis at 100: Historical developments and future directions (pp. 177–203). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  58. Wood, A. M., Maltby, J., Stewart, N., Linley, P. A., & Joseph, S. (2008). A social-cognitive model of trait and state levels of gratitude. Emotion, 8, 281–290.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Zevon, M. A., & Tellegen, A. (1982). The structure of mood change: An idiographic/nomothetic analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 111–122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  • Marieke E. Timmerman
    • 1
  • Eva Ceulemans
  • Anna Lichtwarck-Aschoff
  • Kristof Vansteelandt
  1. 1.Heymans Institute for Psychological ResearchUniversity of GroningenTS GroningenThe Netherlands

Personalised recommendations