Skip to main content

The Unbearable Dynamicity of Psychological Processes: Highlights of the Psychodynamic Theories

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Dynamic Process Methodology in the Social and Developmental Sciences

Abstract

The term dynamic generally refers to the psychology grounded on and informed by psychoanalysis—even if dynamic perspectives do not necessarily coincide with it. It is well known that in Freudian theory, the dynamic level of analysis is that focused on conflicts and their role in shaping psychological facts. Yet contemporary psychoanalytically oriented psychology gives a broader meaning to the label, and consequently dynamic psychology is the psychology concerning the affective source (motivation, instinct, intra-psychic, and/or interpersonal conflicts) shaping (inter)subjectivity. Thus, in contemporary psychology the term psychodynamic can be seen as a synecdoche where the whole—the psychoanalytically oriented psychology—is referred to by means of the part—the dynamic level of analysis as conceptualized by Freud. Here we assume this broad definition. Therefore, henceforth the term psychodynamic will be used as being synonymous with psychoanalytically oriented psychological theory.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    If one assumes, as we do, a non realistic epistemology, it would be more coherent to say that from a theoretical point of view a phenomenon can be denoted as “dynamic” insofar as its manifestations are suited to being depicted accordingly to a model of functioning of such phenomenon having some given defining characteristics. An assertion like this reflects the general epistemological a-ontological assumption according to which any theoretical attribute should not be considered as a description of an intrinsic, essential property of the phenomenon, but rather as an observer’s category usable as a semiotic device in order to encounter the observed (Maturana & Varela, 1980). However, having made this specification here, in order to avoid to weighing down the exposition, we will take it as being valid for the following pages.

  2. 2.

    Obviously, not every psychodynamic theorist shares such a (broadly speaking) developmental look. Psychoanalysis is far from being an unitary domain: As Wallerstein (1998) states, there are many psychoanalyses, not one. Therefore, it should not be surprising to recognize that to some extent a mechanistic point of view is still represented in the psychoanalytic field. It would not be hard to give an example of a psychodynamic theory that sees the phenomenon investigated as an epiphenomenon of an underlying a temporal mechanism.

  3. 3.

    DS specify the properties of the transition function of a dynamic system. Here we will try to rephrase the properties in a less formal way. Consistency implies the existence of a well defined state of the system at all time values and for all admitted input functions; compositionality implies that evolution in time can be described ‘step by step’, taking the state reached at a given time point as starting condition for subsequent computations; causality, implies that if two dynamic systems with identical transition function but starting their evolution from two different initial conditions and different inputs, are found to be in the same state at a given time point, and if they receive the same input thereafter, they will have the same evolution in time, irrespective of the differences due to the initial conditions and previous evolution.

  4. 4.

    In general terms a first order dependence is expressed in the form of a polynomial having variables with low power = 1, i.e., y = ax + b. The typical feature of a linear dependence is that the correspondence between a given increment of the independent variable, Dx, and the corresponding increment of the dependent variable, Dy, is given by a constant, and therefore is not dependent on x.

  5. 5.

    The typical feature of a non-linear dependence on time is that the correspondence between a given increment of the independent variable, DX, and the corresponding increment of the dependent variable, Dt, is not constant. In our example, consider the case of an exponential growth of the density of a bacterial colony in vitro, described by the transition function \(X(t) = X_0 \,\exp (t)\). In this case the population density roughly triples at each time step, with a DX that becomes larger and larger as time elapses.

  6. 6.

    This is indeed a second order linear system as it is the second derivative of the displacement from the rest position \(d^2 x(t)/dt^2 \) that is linearly dependent on x(t), as the equation reads: \(d^2 x(t)/dt^2=- \left( {k/m} \right)x\left( t \right)\).

  7. 7.

    “Chaos theory’s image of patterned complexity offers a far better picture theory (…) to guide our research efforts than does experimental design’s billiard ball determinism image of direct and linear causality. An alternative to experimental studies in psychotherapy is a research approach which recognizes the complexity of the psychotherapeutic process and attempts to analyze the complex unfolding of moment by moment performance of people in specific states and contexts” (Greenberg, 1991, p. 8).

  8. 8.

    Details are described in Salvatore, Tebaldi, and Potì (2009), the study from which the investigation in question is a further development.

  9. 9.

    Firstly, the factorial matrix were segmented according to the 14 blocks of sessions. Secondly, each submatrix was subjected to a factorial analysis. In this way 14 second order factorial matrixes were obtained. Each of them had the first order factorial dimension as a row and the second order factors as a column. This means that the second order factors can be interpreted as the association between first order factors. Finally, each row-vector depicting a given first factorial dimension was transformed into a single value, through the computation of the Euclidean distance of the corresponding point on the phase space defined by the second order factors.

  10. 10.

    A first block corresponds to the initial phase of the psychotherapy (sessions 1–3), a second block corresponds to an early phase (session 13–15), then a middle block (sessions 60–62) and an almost final block (sessions 110–112).

References

  • Amit, J. D. (1989). Modeling brain function. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, P. (1972). More is different. Science, 177, 393–394.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barkham, M., Stiles, W. B., & Shapiro, D. A. (1993). The shape of change in psychotherapy: Longitudinal assessment of personal problems. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 667–677.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bordin, E. S. (1979). The generalizability of the psychoanalytic concept of the working alliance. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 16, 252–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bucci, W. (1998). Psychoanalysis and cognitive science. New York: Guildford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cassidy, J., & Shaver, P. R. (1999). Handbook of attachment: Theory, research and clinical applications. New York: The Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cycle Model (CM) Software. Sektion Informatik in der Psychotherapie, Universität Ulm.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dimaggio, G., & Semerari, A. (2004). Disorganized narratives: The psychological condition and its treatment. In L. E. Angus & J. McLeod (Eds.), The handbook of narrative and psychotherapy. Prectice, theory and research (pp. 263–282). London: Sage Pubblications.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Eco, U. (1976). Theory of semiotics. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., Jurist, E. L., & Target, M. (2002). Affect regulation, mentalization and the development of the self. New York: Other Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fontao, M. I., & Mergenthaler, E. (2007). Application of the therapeuthic cycle model to the research study of micropocesses in group therapy. Revista Argentina de Clínica Psicológica, 14(1), 53–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gabbard, G. (2005). Psychodynamic psychiatry in clinical practice (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gennaro, A., Salvatore, S., Lis, A., & Salcuni, S. (2008, June 18th–22nd). Looking at the psychotherapy process as an intersubjective dynamic of sensemaking. Paper presented at the 39th Annual meeting of the Society of Psychotherapy Research, Barcelona, Spain.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, L. S. (1991). Research on the process of change. Psychotherapy Research, 1, 14–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haken, H. (1992). Synergetics in psycology. In W. Tschacher, G. Schiepek, & J. Brummer (Eds.), Self-organization and clinical psychology. Empirical approaches to synergetics in psychology. Berlin: Springer Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A. M., Laurenceau, J. P., Feldman, G., Strauss, J. L., & Cardaciotto, L. A. (2007). Change is not always linear: The study of nonlinear and discontinuous patterns of change in psychotherapy. Clinical Psychological Review, 27, 715–723.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, I. Z. (1998). Ritual and spontaneity in the psychoanalytic process. New Jersey Hillsdale: The Analytic Press Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hopfield, J. J. (1986). Neural networks and physical systems with emergent collective computational abilities. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 79(21), 2554–2558.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horowitz, M. J., Kernberg, O. F., & Weinshel, E. M. (Eds.). (1993). Psychic structure and psychic change: Essays in honor of Robert Wallerstein. Madison: International University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, D., & Glass, L. (1995). Understanding non linear dynamics. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kowalik, Z., Schiepek, G., Kumpf, K., Roberts, L. E., & Elbert, T. (1997). Psychotherapy as a chaotic process: II. The application of nonlinear analysis methods on quasi time series of the client-therapist interaction: A nonstationary approach. Psychotherapy Research, 7(3), 197–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kraemer, S., Lihl, M., & Mergenthaler, E. (2007). Schlüsselstunden im Verlauf kognitiver Verhaltenstherapie von schizophrenen Patienten: Ein Beitrag zur Prozessforschung. Verhaltenstherapie, 17, 90–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lambert, M. J. (Ed.). (2004). Bergin and Garfield’s handbook of psychotherapy and behavior change (5th ed.). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lambert, M. J., & Barley, D. E. (2001). Research summary on the therapeutic relationship and psychotherapy outcome. Psychotherapy Theory Research Practice and Training, 38, 357–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lancia, F. (in press). Word co-occurrence and similarity in meaning. In S. Salvatore & J. Valsiner (Eds.), Mind as infinite dimensionality. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lauro-Grotto, R., Reich, S., & Virasoro, M. A. (1997). The computational role of conscious processing in a model of semantic memory. In M. Miyashiya, M. Ito, & E. Rolls (Eds.), Cognition, computation and consciousness (pp. 248–263). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lepper, G., & Mergenthaler, E. (2005). Exploring group process. Psychotherapy Research, 15(4), 433–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lepper, G., & Mergenthaler, E. (2007). Therapeutic collaboration: How does it works. Psychotherapy Research, 17(5), 576–587.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, D. J., Garske, J. P., & David, M. K. (2000). Relation of the therapeutic alliance with outcome and other variables: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 438–450.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matte Blanco, I. (1975). The unconscious as infinite sets. London: Gerald Duckworth & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maturana, H. R., & Varela, F. J. (1980). Autopoiesis and cognition: The realization of the living. Dordrecht, Holland: Reidel Publishing Company.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mergenthaler, E. (1996). Emotion abstraction patterns in verbatim protocols: A new way of describing therapeutic processes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 1306–1318.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mergenthaler, E. (1998). CM—the Cycles Model software. (Version 1.0). Sektion Informatik in der Psychotherapie. Ulm, Germany, Universität Ulm.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mergenthaler, E., & Gelo, O. (2007). Un’analisi qualitativa del Disturbo di Personalità Narcisista attraverso il Modello del Ciclo Terapeutico: uno studio single-case (Caso K.). In G. Nicolò & S. Salvatore (Eds.), La ricerca sui risultati e sul processo in psicoterapia (pp. 313–328). Roma: Edizioni Carlo Amore.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mezard, M., Parisi, G., & Virasoro, M. A. (1987). Spin glass theory and beyond. Singapore: World Scientific.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mossi, P.G., & Salvatore (in press) Psychological transition from meaning to sense

    Google Scholar 

  • Odgen, T. H. (2004). The analytic third: Implications for psychoanalytic theory and technique. The Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 73(1), 167–194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Orsucci, F. (2006). The paradigm of complexity in clinical neuro-cognitive science. The Neuroscientist, 12(5), 390–397.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ramseyer, F., & Tschacher, W. (2006). Synchrony a core concept for a constructivist approach to psychotherapy. Constructivism in the Human Sciences, 11, 150–171.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rapaport, D. (1960). The structure of psychoanalytic theory. A systematizing attempt. New York: International Universities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rommetveit, R. (1992). Outlines of a dialogically based social-cognitive approach to human cognition and communication. In A. H. Wold (Ed.), The dialogical alternative towards a theory of language and mind (pp. 19–44). Oslo: Scandinavian University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Safran, J. D., & Muran, J. C. (2000). Negotiating the therapeutic alliance: A relational treatment guide. New York: Guildford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salvatore, S., Gelo, O., Gennaro, A., & Manzo, S., & Al-Radaideh, A. (in press). Looking at the psychotherapy process as an intersubjective dynamics of meaning making. A case study with Discourse Flow Analysis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salvatore, S., Lauro-Grotto, R., Gennaro, A., & Gelo, O. (2009). Attempts to grasp the dynamicity of intersubjectivity. In J. Valsiner, P. C. M. Molenaar, M. C. D. P. Lyra, & N. Chaudhary (Eds.), Dynamic process methodology in the social and developmental sciences (pp. 171–190). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salvatore, S., Quarta R., & Ruggeri R. (in press). The effect of violent videogames in eliciting aggressiveness. A semiotic model.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salvatore, S., Tebaldi, C., & Potì, S. (2009). The discoursive dynamic of sensemaking. In S. Salvatore, J. Valsiner, S. Strout, & J. Clegg (Eds.), Yearbook of idiographic science 2008 (Vol. 1). Rome: Firera Publishing Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salvatore, S., & Valsiner, J. (2006). “Am I really a psychologist?”. “Making sense of a super-human social role”. European Journal of School Psychology, 4(2), 127–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salvatore, S., & Venuleo, C. (2008). Understanding the role of emotion in sense-making. A semiotic psychoanalytic oriented perspective. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 42(1), 32–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salvatore, S., & Venuleo, C. (2009). The unconscious as source of sense: A psychodynamic approach to meaning. In B. Wagoner (Ed.), Symbolic transformations: The mind in movement through culture and society. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sato, T. (2009). Constructing life course trajectories. In J. Valsiner, P. C. M. Moolenar, M. C. D. P. Lyra, & N. Chaudhary (Eds.), Dynamic process methodology in the social and developmental sciences (pp. 217–240). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sato, T., Yasuda, Y., Kido, A., Arakava, A., Mizoguchi, H., & Valsiner, J. (2007). Sampling reconsidered: Idiographic scienze and the analysis of personal life trajectories. In J. Valsiner, A. Rosa (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of sociocultural psychology (pp. 82–106). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiepek, G., Kiwalik, Z. J., Schutz, A., & Kohler, M. (1997). Psychotherapy as a chaotic process: I. Coding the client-therapist interaction by means of sequential plan analysis and the search for chaos: A stationary approach. Psychotherapy Research, 7(3),173–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schiepek, G., Tschacher, W., & Kaimel , E. (1992). Self organization and clinical psychology. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Semerari, A., Carcione, A., Dimaggio, G., Nicolò, G., & Procacci, M. (2007). Understanding minds: Different functions and different disorders? The Contribution of Psychotherapy Research. Psychotherapy Research, 7(1), 106–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sève, L. (1972). The structural method and the dialectical method. International Journal of Sociology, 2, 2–3.

    Google Scholar 

  • Storolow, R. D., Atwood, G. E., & Brandchaft, B. (1994). The intersubjective perspective. Hillsdale, NJ: Jason Aronson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strogatz, S. H. (1994). Nonlinear dynamics and chaos, with applications to physics, biology, chemistry and engineering. Cambridge, MA: Perseus Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tschacher, W., Ramseyer, F., & Grawe, K. (2007). Der Ordnungseffekt im Psychotherapieprozess Replikation einer systemtheoretischen Vorhersage und Zusammenhang mit dem Therapieerfolg. Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie und Psychotherapie, 36, 18–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tscacher, W., Scheier, C., & Grawe, K. (1998). Order and pattern formation in psychotherapy. Non linear dynamics psychology and life sciences, 2, 195–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tschacher, W., Schiepek, G., & Brummer, E. J. (Eds.). (1992). Self-organization and clinical psychology. Empirical approaches to synergetics in psychology. Berlin: Springer Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (2007). Culture in minds and societies: Foundations of cultural psychology. New Delhi: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Venuleo, C., Salvatore, S., Mossi, P. G., Grassi, R., & Ruggeri, R. (2008). The didactic relationship in the changing world. Outlines for a theory of the reframing setting. European Journal of School Psychology, 5(2), 151–180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallerstein, R. S. (1998). One psychoanalysis or Many? The International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 69, 5–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Westen, D. (1998). The scientifc legacy of Sigmund Freud: Toward a psychodynamically informed psychological science. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 333–371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1958). Philosophische Untersuchungen. Oxford: Basic Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgement

We wish to thank the participants in the session of the Clark University’s Kitchen Seminar (10 September, 2008) devoted to the discussion of a first draft of this text. The discussion that developed there provided meaningful feedback, helping us to bring the implications of our proposal into focus.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Rosapia Lauro-Grotto .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2009 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Lauro-Grotto, R., Salvatore, S., Gennaro, A., Gelo, O. (2009). The Unbearable Dynamicity of Psychological Processes: Highlights of the Psychodynamic Theories. In: Valsiner, J., Molenaar, P., Lyra, M., Chaudhary, N. (eds) Dynamic Process Methodology in the Social and Developmental Sciences. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-95922-1_1

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics