Advertisement

Anger in Children’s Tantrums: A New, Quantitative, Behaviorally Based Model

  • Michael Potegal
  • Peihua Qiu
Chapter

Abstract

Because excessive anger in early childhood can predict later psychopathology, quantifying its intensity and time course is clinically important. Anger consists of a set of experiential, physiological, and behavioral responses whose coherence is sufficient to justify the assumption of a common latent variable that can vary in intensity. The relationships between anger intensity and various anger-driven behaviors in children’s tantrums are probabilistic, nonlinear, and different for each individual behavior. Although any one behavior can provide only a partial and indirect measure of anger intensity, the entire trajectory of anger across the tantrum may be reconstructed by combining the observed temporal distributions of the various behaviors. In particular, we observed that behaviors characteristic of lower intensities of anger tend to occur at both the beginning and the end of tantrums while behaviors linked to higher intensities of anger are distributed around a single early peak. Accordingly, our anger intensity-behavioral linkage function model reconstructs a single, common, latent anger intensity variable, MA(t), whose rise and fall controls the momentary probability of eight angry tantrum behaviors through linkage functions that are unique to each behavior. We introduce the MA50 as a practical measure of the “characteristic” intensity of the eight angry behaviors and note how the model may inform study of the neural substrates of anger.

Keywords

Linkage Function Facial Expression Anger Expression Display Rule High Anger 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Notes

Acknowledgments

Collection of the Wisconsin data was supported by a grant to M. Potegal from the Harry Frank Guggenheim Foundation and by National Research Service Awards to M. Potegal from the National Institute for Neurological Disorders and Stroke (F33 NS09638) and the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (F33 HD08208). At that time, the first author was a Fellow in the laboratory of Richard J. Davidson, where work was supported in part by an NIMH Center for Behavioral Sciences Research Grant (P50-MH52354) to the Wisconsin Center for Affective Neuroscience (R.J. Davidson, Director) and by an NIMH Research Scientist Award (KO5-MH00875). Collection of the Minnesota data and development of the anger intensity-linkage function model were supported by grants to M. Potegal from the National Institute for Mental Health (R03-MH58739) and from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (R21 HD048426); this second stage was also supported by a Grant-in-Aid from University of Minnesota Graduate Faculties (Emotion and manipulation in toddler tantrums: Behavior/cortisol relations), the Viking Children’s Fund (643–7257) and an equipment grant from the Minnesota Medical Foundation.

We thank the University of Minnesota undergraduates who coded the various Minnesota tantrum records including Vanessa Downs, Ross Oden, and Jennifer B. Swanson. We thank Kristin Buss, Nancy Eisenberg, James Green, Jan Hoeksma, Jill Kilderman, Alicia Kunin-Batson, Harriet Oster, and Cynthia Stifter for their critical reading of earlier versions of this paper, in part or in whole. David Mottet improved the graphics of Fig. 12.2.

References

  1. Alvarado, N., & Jameson, K. A. (2002). Varieties of anger: The relation between emotion terms and components of anger expression. Motivation and Emotion, 26, 153–182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Archer, J., & Haigh, A. (1999). Sex differences in beliefs about aggression: Opponent’s sex and the form of aggression. British Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 71–84.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Astin, S., Redston, P., & Campbell, A. (2003). Sex differences in social representations of aggression: Men justify, women excuse? Aggressive Behavior, 29, 128–133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Blanchard, D. C., & Blanchard, R. J. (2008). Defensive behaviors, fear, and anxiety. In R. J. Blanchard, D. C. Blanchard, G. Greibel, & D. J. Nutt (Eds.), Handbook of anxiety and fear (pp. 63–80). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bonano, G. A., & Keltner, D. (2004). The coherence of emotion systems: Comparing “on-line” measures of appraisal, facial expressions, and self-report. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 431–444.Google Scholar
  6. Campbell, A., & Muncer, S. (1994). Sex differences in aggression: social representation and social roles. British Journal of Social Psychology, 33, 233–240.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Camras, L., Sullivan, J., & Michel, G. (1993). Do infants express discrete emotions? Adult judgments of facial, vocal and body actions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 17, 171–186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Casey, R. (1993). Children’s emotional experience: Relations among expression, self-report, and understanding. Developmental Psychology, 29, 119–129.Google Scholar
  9. Coan, J. A., & Allen, J. B. (2003). Varieties of emotional experience during voluntary emotional facial expressions. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 1000, 375–379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Cole, P. M., Teti, L. O., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (2003). Mutual emotion regulation and the stability of conduct problems between preschool and early school age. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 1–18.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Cook, R. D., & Weisberg, S. (1999). Applied regression including computing and graphics. New York: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Cope, T. C., & Pinter, M. J. (1995). The size principal of motorneuron recruitment: Still working after alI these years. News in Physiological Sciences, 10, 280–286.Google Scholar
  13. Davidson, R. J. (1992). Prolegomenon to the structure of emotion: Gleanings from neuropsychology. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 245–268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Davis, T. L. (1995). Gender differences in masking negative emotions: Ability or motivation? Developmental Psychology, 31, 660–667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Dearing, K. F., Hubbard, J. A., Ramsden, S. R., Parker, E. H., Relyea, N., Smithmyer, C. M., et al. (2002). Children’s self-reports about anger regulation: Direct and indirect links to social preference and aggression. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 48, 308–335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Deffenbacher, J. L., Oetting, E. R., Lynch, R. S., & Morris, C. (1996). The expression of anger and its consequences. Behavior Research and Therapy, 34, 575–590.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Denham, S., & Couchoud, E. (1990). Young preschoolers understanding of emotions. Child Study Journal, 20, 171–191.Google Scholar
  18. Dobash, R. E., & Dobash, R. P. (1984). The nature and antecedents of violent events. British Journal of Criminology, 24, 269–288.Google Scholar
  19. Eckhardt, C. I., & Deffenbacher, J. L. (1995). Diagnosis of anger disorders. In H. Kassinove (Eds.), Anger disorders: Definition, diagnosis and treatment (pp. 27–48). Washington, DC: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
  20. Edwards, P. (1998). Etude empirique de déterminants de la différenciation des émotions et de leur intensité. [An empirical study of the determinants of the differentiation and the intensity of the emotions.] Ph.D. Thesis. University of Geneva, Switzerland.Google Scholar
  21. Einon, D. F., & Potegal, M. (1994). Temper tantrums in young children. In M. Potegal & J. Knutson (Eds.), Dynamics of aggression: Biological and social processes in dyads and groups (pp. 157–194). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Assoc.Google Scholar
  22. Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Murphy, B. C., Shaepard, S., Guthrie, I. K., Maszk, P., et al. (1999). Prediction of elementary school children’s socially appropriate and problem behavior from anger reactions at 4–6 years. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 20, 119–142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Ekman, P. (1994). Strong evidence for universals in facial expressions: A reply to Russell’s mistaken critique. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 268–287.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Fanselow, M. S. (1994). Neural organization of the defensive behavior system responsible for fear. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 429–438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Fernandez-Dols, J.-M., Sanchez, F., Carrera, P., & Ruiz-Belda, M.-A. (1997). Are spontaneous expressions and emotions linked? An experimental test of coherence. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 21, 163–177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Feshbach, S. (1964). The function of aggression and the regulation of aggressive drive. Psychological Review, 71, 257–272.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Freud, A. (1971). Comments on aggression. International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 53, 163–171.Google Scholar
  28. Fridlund, A. J. (1997). The new ethology of human facial expressions. In J. A. Russell & J. M. Fernandez-Dols (Eds.), The psychology of facial expression (pp. 103–129). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  29. Frijda, N. H., Mesquita, B., Sonnemans, J., & Van Goozen, S. (1991). The duration of affective phenomena or emotions, sentiments, and passions. In K. T. Strongman (Eds.). International review of studies of emotion (Vol. 1, pp. 187–226). New York: Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
  30. Frijda, N. H., Ortony, A., Sonnemans, J., & Clore, G. (1992). The complexity of intensity. In M. Clark et al. (Eds.), Emotion review of personality and social psychology (pp. 60–89), 13 Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
  31. Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Review of General Psychology, 2, 271–299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Haidt, J., & Keltner, D. (1999). Culture and facial expression: Open-ended methods find more expressions and a gradient of recognition. Cognition and Emotion, 13, 225–266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Harmon-Jones, E., Sigelman, J. D., Bohlig, A., & Harmon-Jones, C. (2003). Anger, coping, and frontal cortical activity: The effect of coping potential on anger-induced left frontal activity. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 1–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Hess, U. (2001). The communication of emotion. In A. Kaszniak (Ed.), Emotions, qualia and consciousness (pp. 397–409). Singapore: World Scientific Publishing.Google Scholar
  35. Hess, U., Blairy, S., & Kleck, R. (1997). The intensity of emotional facial expressions and decoding accuracy. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 21, 241–257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Hoeksma, J. B., Oosterlaan, J., & Schipper, E. (2004). Emotion regulation and the dynamics of feelings: A conceptual and methodological framework. Child Development, 75, 354–360.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Hubbard, J. A., Parker, E. H., Ramsden, S. R., Flanagan, K. D., Nicole Relyea, N., Dearing, K. F., et al. (2004). The relations among observational, physiological, and self-report measures of children’s anger. Social Development, 13, 14–39.Google Scholar
  38. Huesmann, L. R., Eron, L. D., Lefkowitz, M. M., & Walder, L. O. (1984). Stability of aggression over time and generations. Developmental Psychology, 20, 1120–1134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Knight, G. P., Guthries, I. K., Page, M. C., & Fabes, R. A. (2002). Emotional arousal and gender differences in aggression: A meta-analysis. Aggressive Behavior, 28, 366–393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Kuppens, P., Mechelen, I., & Van Meulders, M. (2004). Every cloud has a silver lining: interpersonal and individual differences determinants of anger-related behaviors. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1550–1564.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Leibenluft, E., Charney, D. S., Towbin, K. E., Bhangoo, R. K., & Pine, D. S. (2003). Defining clinical phenotypes of juvenile mania. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 430–437.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Levine, L., Stein, N. L., & Liwag, M. D. (1999). Remembering children’s emotions: Sources of concordant and discordant accounts between parents and children. Developmental Psychology, 35, 790–801.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Liang, K. Y., & Zeger, S. L. (1986). Longitudinal analysis using generalized linear models. Biometrika, 73, 13–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Mascolo, M. F., Harkins, D., & Harakal, T. (2000). The dynamic construction of emotion: Varieties of anger. In M. D. Lewis & I. Granic (Eds.), Emotion, development & self-organization (pp. 125–154). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Matsumoto, D. (1989). Face, culture, and judgements of anger and fear: Do the eyes have it? Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 13, 171–188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Matsumoto, D., Nezlek, B., & Koopmann, B. (2007). Evidence for universality in phenomenological emotion response system coherence. Emotion, 7, 57–67.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. McNaughton, N., & Corr, P. J. (2004). A two-dimensional neuropsychology of defense: Fear/anxiety and defensive distance. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 28, 285–305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Mikula, G., Scherer, K. R., & Athenstaedt, U. (1998). The role of injustice in the elicitation of differential emotional reactions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24, 769–783.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Mirowsky, J., & Ross, C. E. (1995). Sex differences in distress: Real or artifact? American Sociological Review, 60, 449–468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Mobbs, D., Petrovic, P., Marchant, J. L., Hassabis, D., Nikolaus Weiskopf, N., Seymour, B., et al. (2007). When fear is near: Threat imminence elicits prefrontal-periaqueductal gray shifts in humans. Science, 317, 1079–1083.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Nagin, D. S., & Tremblay, R. E. (2001). Parental and early childhood predictors of persistent physical aggression in boys from kindergarten to high school. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 389–394.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Orobio de Castro, B., Merk, W., Koops, W., Veerma, J. W., & Bosch, J. D. (2005). Emotions in social information processing and their relations with reactive and proactive aggression in referred aggressive boys. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 34, 105–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Parens, H. (1993). Rage toward the self and others in early childhood. In R. A. Glick & S. P. Roose (Eds.). Rage, power, and aggression. The role of affect in motivation, development, and adaptation (Vol. 2, pp. 123–147). New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
  54. Plutchik, R. (1980). Emotion: A psychoevolutionary synthesis. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
  55. Potegal, M. (2003, April). Preliminary observations on salivary cortisol baseline and transients associated with tantrums in 3 year olds. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development Society for Research in Child Development, Tampa, FL.Google Scholar
  56. Potegal, M. (2005, April). Tantrums in externalizing, internalizing and typically developing 4 year olds. Poster presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development: Atlanta, GA.Google Scholar
  57. Potegal, M., Robison, S., Anderson, F., Jordan, C., & Shapiro, E. (2007). Sequence and priming in 15 month olds’ reactions to brief arm restraint: Evidence for a hierarchy of anger responses. Aggressive Behavior, 33, 1–11.Google Scholar
  58. Potegal, M., & Archer, J. (2004). Sex differences in childhood anger and aggression. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America: Sex and Gender, 13, 513–528.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Potegal, M., Carlson, G., Margulies, D., Gutkovitch, Z., & Wall, M. (2009). Rages or temper tantrums? The behavioral organization, temporal characteristics, and clinical significance of angry-agitated outbursts in child psychiatry inpatients. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 40, 621–636.Google Scholar
  60. Potegal, M., & Davidson, R. J. (2003). Temper tantrums in young children: (1) Behavioral composition. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 24, 140–147.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Potegal, M., Kosorok, M. R., & Davidson, R. J. (1996). The time course of angry behavior in the temper tantrums of young children. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 794, 31–45.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Potegal, M., Kosorok, M. R., & Davidson, R. (2003). Temper tantrums in young children: (2) Tantrum duration and temporal organization. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 24, 148–154.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Qiu, P., Yang, R., & Potegal, M. (2005). Generalized nonlinear modeling of the time course of tantrum anger. Technical Report, School of Statistics, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN 55455.Google Scholar
  64. Qiu, P., Yang, R., & Potegal, M. (2009). Statistical modeling of the time course of tantrum anger Annals of Applied Statistics, 3, 1013–1034.Google Scholar
  65. Radke-Yarrow, M., & Kochanska, G. (1990). Anger in young children. In N. L. Stein, B. Levinthal, & T. Trabasso (Eds.), Psychological and biological approaches to emotion (pp. 297–310). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  66. Ramirez, J. M., Andreu, J. M., & Fujihara, T. (2001). Cultural and sex differences in aggression: A comparison between Japanese and Spanish students using two different inventories. Aggressive Behavior, 27, 313–322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Richardson, D. S., & Huguet, P. (2001). Beliefs about and experience with aggression in the United States and France. In J. M. Ramirez & D. S. Richardson (Eds.), Cross-cultural approaches to aggression and reconciliation (pp. 73–85). New York: Nova Science.Google Scholar
  68. Roseman, I. J., Wiest, C., & Swartz, T. S. (1994). Phenomenology, behaviors and goals differentiate discrete emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 206–221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Rosenberg, E., & Ekman, P. (1994). Coherence between expressive and experiential systems in emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 8, 201–229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Ross, C. E., & Van Willigen, M. (1996). Gender, parenthood, and anger. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 58, 572–584.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Russell, J. A. (1995). Facial expressions of emotion: What lies beyond minimal universality? Psychological Bulletin, 118, 379–391.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Russell, J. A. (1997). Reading emotions from and into faces: Resurrecting a dimensional-contextual perspective. In J. Russell & J. M. Fernandez-Dols (Eds.), The psychology of facial expression (pp. 295–320). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Saarni, C. (1993). Socialization of emotion. In M. Lewis & J. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotion (pp. 435–446). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  74. Shaver, P., Wu, S., & Schwartz, J. C. (1992). Cross-cultural similarities and differences in emotion and its representation: A prototype approach. In M. Clark (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology Vol 13: Emotion (pp. 175–212). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  75. Shipman, K. L., Zeman, J., Nesin, A. E., & Fitzgerald, M. (2003). Children’s strategies for displaying anger and sadness: What works with whom? Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 49, 100–122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Snyder, J., Stoolmiller, M., Wilson, M., & Yamamoto, M. (2003). Child anger regulation, parental responses to children’s anger displays, and early child antisocial behavior. Social Development, 12, 335–360.Google Scholar
  77. Sonnemans, J., & Frijda, N. H. (1994). The structure of subjective emotional intensity. Cognition and Emotion, 4, 329–350.Google Scholar
  78. Stein, N. L., Trabasso, T., & Liwag, M. (1993). The representation and organization of emotional experience: Unfolding the emotion episode. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 279–300). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  79. Stemmler, G. (1997). Selective activation of traits: Boundary conditions for the activation of anger. Personality and Individual Differences, 22, 213–233.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Tassinanry, L. G., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1992). Unobservable facial actions and emotion. Psychological Science, 3, 28–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Underwood, M. K., & Bjornstad, G. B. (2001). Children’s emotional experience of peer provocation: The relation between observed behaviour and self-reports of emotions, expressions, and social goals. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 25, 320–330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Underwood, M. K., Coie, J. D., & Herbsman, C. R. (1992). Display rules for anger and aggression in school-age children. Child Development, 63, 366–380.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Vingerhoets, A., & Scheirs, J. (2000). Sex differences in crying: Empirical findings and possible explanations. In A. H. Fischer (Ed.), Gender and emotion (pp. 143–165). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Wallbott, H. (1998). Bodily expression of emotion. European Journal of Social Psychology, 28, 879–896.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. Winstok, Z. (2007). Perceptions, emotions, and behavioral decisions in conflicts that escalate to violence. Motivation and Emotion, 31, 125–136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. Zeichner, A., Parrott, D. J., & Frey, F. C. (2003). Gender differences in laboratory aggression under response choice conditions. Aggressive Behavior, 29, 95–106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.University of MinnesotaMinneapolisUSA

Personalised recommendations