The Development of Anger



This chapter describes the development of anger. To do so I need to distinguish between anger and other emotions and behaviors that are often confused with it. Having done so, I will argue that anger is an approach emotion. Unlike sadness or fear and more like happiness, its function is to move the infant into active engagement in its world in order to overcome obstacles to desired goals. Anger, as an approach action pattern, exists early and can be seen within the first few months of life.


Heart Rate Variability Behavioral Inhibition Cortisol Response Anger Expression Cortisol Release 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.


  1. Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  2. Alessandri, S. M., Sullivan, M. W., Imaizumi, S., & Lewis, M. (1993). Learning and emotional responsivity in cocaine-exposed infants. Developmental Psychology, 29(6), 989–997.Google Scholar
  3. Alessandri, S. M., Sullivan, M. W., & Lewis, M. (1990). Violation of expectancy and frustration in early infancy. Developmental Psychology, 26(5), 738–744.Google Scholar
  4. Allen, J. B. (2005). Cardiac vagal tone, defensiveness, and motivational style. Biological Psychology, 68(2), 147–162.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Ballespi, S., Jane, M. C., Riba, M. D., & Domenech-Liaberia, E. (2002). Analysis of anxious and depressive symptoms of a sample of inhibited preschoolers. Revista de Psiquiatria Infanto-Juvenil, 19(4), 184–191.Google Scholar
  6. Bauer, A. M., Quas, J. A., & Boyce, W. T. (2002). Associations between physiological reactivity and children’s behavior: Advantages of a multisystem approach. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 23(2), 102–113.Google Scholar
  7. Bazhenova, O., Plonskaia, O., & Porges, S. W. (2001). Vagal reactivity and affective adjustment in infants during interaction challenges. Child Development, 72, 1314–1326.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Belsky, J. (1997). Theory, testing, effect-size evaluation, and differential susceptibility to rearing influence: The case of mothering and attachment. Child Development, 64, 598–600.Google Scholar
  9. Berston, G. G., Cacioppo, J. T., & Quigley, K. S. (1993). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia: Autonomic origins, physiological mechanisms and psychological implications. Psychophysiology, 30, 183–196.Google Scholar
  10. Blair, C., Peters, R., & Granger, D. (2004). Physiological and neuropsychological correlates of approach/withdrawal tendencies in preschool: Further examination of the behavioral inhibition system/behavioral activation system scales for young children. Developmental Psychobiology, 45(3), 113–124.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Braungart-Reiker, J. M., & Stifter, C. (1996). Infant responses to frustrating situations. Continuity and change in reactivity and regulation. Child Development, 67, 1767–1779.Google Scholar
  12. Bridges, K. L. (1932). Emotional development in early infancy. Child Development, 3, 324–341.Google Scholar
  13. Brinker, R., & Lewis, M. (1982). Making the world work: A learning prosthesis for handicapped infants. Exceptional Children, 49, 163–170.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Brown, M. S., & Tanner, C. (1988). Type A behavior and cardiovascular responsivity in preschoolers. Nursing Research, 37(3), 152–155.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Buss, K. A., Davidson, R. J., Kalin, N. H., & Goldsmith, H. H. (2004). Context-specific freezing and associated physiological reactivity as a dysregulated fear response. Developmental Psychology, 40(4), 583–594.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Buss, K. A., & Goldsmith, H. H. (1998). Fear and anger regulation in infancy: Effects on the temporal dynamics of affective expression. Child Development, 69, 359–374.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Buss, K. A., & Kiel, E. J. (2004). Comparison of sadness, anger, and fear facial expressions when toddlers look at their mothers. Child Development, 75(6), 1761–1773.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Buss, K. A., Schumacher, J. W., Dolski, I., Kalin, N. H., & Davidson, R. J. (2003). Right frontal brain activity, cortisol, and withdrawal behavior in 6-month-old infants. Behavioral Neuroscience, 117(1), 11–20.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Cacioppo, J. T., Klein, D. J., Berntson, G. G., & Hatfield, E. (1993). The psychophysiology of emotion. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotion (pp. 119–142). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  20. Calkins, S. D. (2009). Regulatory competence and early disruptive behavior problems: The role of physiological regulation. In S. Olson, & A. Sameroff (Eds.), Biopyschosocial regulatory processes in the development of childhood behavioral problems (pp. 86–115) NY, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  21. Calkins, S. D., & Fox, N. (1992). The relations among infant temperament, security of attachment, and behavioral inhibition at twenty-four months. Child Development, 63, 1456–1472.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Calkins, S. D., Graziano, P. A., & Keane, S. P. (2007). Cardiac vagal regulation differentiates among children at risk for behavior problems. Biological Psychology, 74(2), 144–153.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Calkins, S. D., & Johnson, M. C. (1998). Toddler regulation of distress to frustrating events:Temperamental and maternal correlates. Infant Behavior and Development, 21(3), 379–395.Google Scholar
  24. Calkins, S. D., & Keane, S. P. (2004). Cardiac vagal regulation across the preschool period: Stability, continuity, and implications for childhood adjustment. Developmental Psychobiology, 45, 101–112.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Calkins, S. D., Smith, C. L., Gill, K. L., & Johnson, M. C. (1998). Maternal interactive style across contexts: Relations to emotional behavioral, and physiological regulation during toddlerhood. Social Development, 7(3), 350–369.Google Scholar
  26. Camras, L. (2004). An event-emotion or event-expression hypothesis? A comment on the commentaries on Bennett, Bendersky, and Lewis (2002). Infancy, 6(3), 431–433.Google Scholar
  27. Camras, L., Oster, H., Campos, J. J., Miyake, K., & Bradshaw, D. (1992). Japanese and American infants’ responses to arm restraint. Developmental Psychology, 28(4), 578–583.Google Scholar
  28. Carver, C. S. (2004). Negative affects deriving from the behavioral approach system. Emotion, 4(1), 3–22.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Carver, C. S., Sutton, S. K., & Scheier, M. F. (2000). Action, emotion, and personality: Emerging conceptual integration. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(6), 741–751.Google Scholar
  30. Coan, J. A., Allen, J. J. B., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2001). Voluntary facial expression and hemispheric asymmetry over the frontal cortex. Psychophysiology, 38(6), 912–925.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. Chicago: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
  32. Darwin, C. (1965). The expression of emotion in man and animals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  33. Davidson, R. (1998). Affective style and affective disorders: Perspectives from neuroscience. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 307–330.Google Scholar
  34. DeCapser, A. J., & Carstens, A. A. (1981). Contingencies of stimulation: Effects of learning and motivation. Infant Behavior and Development, 4, 19–35.Google Scholar
  35. Dichter-Blancher, T. B., Busch-Rossnagel, N. A., & Knauf-Jensen, D. E. (1997). Mastery motivation: Appropriate tasks for toddlers. Infant Behavior and Development, 20, 545–548.Google Scholar
  36. Doussard-Roosevelt, J. A. (2003). Short-term stability of physiological measures in kindergarten children: Respiratory sinus arrhythmia, heart period, and cortisol. Developmental Psychobiology, 43(3), 231–242.Google Scholar
  37. Dweck, C., Chiu, C., & Hong, Y. (1995). Implicit theories: Implicit theories and their role in judgments and reactions: A world from two perspectives. Psychological Inquiry, 6, 267–285.Google Scholar
  38. Eisenberg, N., Qing, A., Spinrad, T. L., Valiente, C., Fabes, R. A., & Liew, J. (2005). Relations among positive parenting, children’s effortful control and externalizing problems: A three wave longitudinal study. Child Development, 76(5), 1055–1071.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Field, T. (1994). The effects of mother’s physical and emotional unavailability on emotion regulation. In N. A. Fox (Ed.), Monographs of the society for research on child development (Vol. 59, Serial No. 240, pp. 208–227). Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  40. Fox, N., & Calkins, S. D. (1993). Pathways to aggression and social withdrawal: Interactions among temperament, attachment, and regulation. In K. H. Rubin & J. Asendorf (Eds.), Social withdrawal, shyness and inhibition in childhood (pp. 81–100). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  41. Fox, N., & Davidson, R. (1988). Patterns of brain electrical activity during facial signs of emotion in 10-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 24(2), 230–236.Google Scholar
  42. Fox, N., Henderson, H. A., Rubin, K. H., Calkins, S. D., & Schmidt, L. A. (2001). Continuity and discontinuity of behavioral inhibition and exuberance: Psychophysiological and behavioral influences across the first four years of life. Child Development, 72, 1–21.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Fox, N., Kimmerly, N. L., & Schafer, W. D. (1991). Attachment to mother, attachment to father: A meta-analysis. Child Development, 62, 210–225.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  44. Frances, D., Diorio, J., LaPlante, P., Weaver, S., Secki, J. R., & Meaney, M. J. (1996). The role of early environmental events in regulating neuroendocrine development: Moms, pups, stress, and glucocorticoid receptors. Annals of the New York Academey of Sciences, 794, 136–152.Google Scholar
  45. Freud, S. ([1914]1957). On narcissism: An Introduction (Standard Ed., Vol. 14, pp. 67–102). London: Hogarth Press.Google Scholar
  46. Goldsmith, H. H., & Alansky, J. A. (1987). Maternal and infant temperamental predictors of attachment: A meta-analytic view. Journal of Clinical and Consulting Psychology, 55, 805–816.Google Scholar
  47. Gotlib, I. H., Joormann, J., Minor, K. L., & Cooney, R. E. (2006). Cognitive and biological functioning in children at risk for depression. In T. Canli (Ed.), Biology of personality and individual differences. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  48. Gunnar, M. R., Morison, S. J., Chisholm, K., & Schuder, M. (2001). Salivary cortisol levels in children adopted from Romanian orphanages. Development and Psychopathology, 13, 611–628.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Harmon-Jones, E. (2003). Clarifying the emotive functions of asymmetrical frontal cortical activity. Psychophysiology, 40(6), 838–848.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Harmon-Jones, E. (2004). Contributions from research on anger and cognitive dissonance to understanding the motivational functions of asymmetrical frontal brain activity. Biological Psychology, 67(1–2), 51–76.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Hart, J., Gunnar, M. R., & Cicchetti, D. (1995). Salivary cortisol in maltreated children: Evidence of relations between neuroendocrine activity and social competence. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 11–26.Google Scholar
  52. Hennessy, M. B., Deak, T., & Schiml-Webb, P. A. (2001). Stress-induced sickness behaviors: An alternative hypothesis for responses during maternal separation. Developmental Psychobiology, 39(2), 76–83.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Hennessy, M. B., O’Leary, S. K., Hawke, J. L., & Wilson, S. E. (2002). Social influences on cortisol and behavioral responses of preweaning, periadolescent, and adult guinea pigs. Physiology & Behavior, 76, 305–314.Google Scholar
  54. Henry, J. P. (1992). Biological basis of the stress response. Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Science, 27, 66–83.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Hewig, J., Hagemann, D., Seifert, J., Naumann, E., & Bartussek, D. (2004). On the selective relation of frontal cortical asymmetry and anger-out versus anger-control. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 87(6), 926–939.Google Scholar
  56. Hofheimer, J. A., Wood, B. R., Porges, S W, Pearson, E., & Lawson, E. E. (1995). Respiratory sinus arrhythmia and social interaction patterns in preterm newborns. Infant Behavior and Development, 18, 233–245.Google Scholar
  57. Huot, R. L., Brennan, P. A., Stowe, Z. N., Plotsky, P. M., & Walker, E. F. (2004). Negative affect in offspring of depressed mothers is predicted by infant cortisol levels at 6 months and maternal depression during pregnancy but not postpartum. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1. Biobehavioral Stress Response: Protective and Damaging Effects, 1032, 234–236.Google Scholar
  58. Izard, C. E. (1977). Human emotions. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  59. Izard, C. E., Hembree, E. A., & Heubner, R. R. (1987). Infants’ emotional expressions to acute pain: Developmental change and stability of individual differences. Developmental Psychology, 23, 105–113.Google Scholar
  60. Jennings, K. D. (2004). Development of goal-directed behaviour and related self-processes in toddlers. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 28(4), 319–327.Google Scholar
  61. Kagan, J. (1994). Galen’s prophecy. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  62. Kagan, J., Reznick, J. S., & Snidman, N. (1987). The physiology and psychology of behavioral inhibition in children. Child Development, 58, 1459–1473.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Kerr, M., Tremblay, R., Pagani, L., & Vitaro, F. (1997). Boys’ behavioral inhibition and the risk of later delinquency. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54(9), 809–816.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Kohut, H. (1972). Thoughts on narcissism and narcissistic rage. The Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 27, 360–400.Google Scholar
  65. Lerner, J. S., Gonzalez, R. M., Dahl, R. E., Hariri, A. R., & Taylor, S. E. (2005). Facial expressions of emotion reveal neuroendocrine and cardiovascular stress responses. Biological Psychiatry, 58, 743–750.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  66. Levine, S., Coe, C., & Weiner, S. G. (1989). Psychoneuroendocrinology of stress: A psychobiological perspective. In F. R. Bush & S. Levine (Eds.), Psychoendocrinology (pp. 341–377). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  67. Lewis, M. (1990). The development of intentionality and the role of consciousness. Psychological Inquiry, 1(3), 231–247.Google Scholar
  68. Lewis, M. (1992). Shame: The exposed self. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
  69. Lewis, M. (1993). The development of anger and rage. In R. A. Glick & S. P. Roose (Eds.), Rage, power, and aggression: The role of affect in motivation, development and adaptation. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
  70. Lewis, M. (1995). Shame: The exposed self (Paperback edition). New York: The Free Press.Google Scholar
  71. Lewis, M., Alessandri, S. M., & Sullivan, M. W. (1990). Violation of expectancy, loss of control, and anger expressions in young infants. Developmental Psychology, 26(5), 745–751.Google Scholar
  72. Lewis, M., & Goldberg, S. (1969). Perceptual-cognitive development in infancy: A generalized expectancy model as a function of mother-infant interaction. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 15, 81–100.Google Scholar
  73. Lewis, M., Hitchcock, D. F. A., & Sullivan, M. W. (2004). Physiological and emotional reactivity to learning and frustration. Infancy, 6(1), 121–143.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Lewis, M., & Michalson, L. (1983). Children’s emotions and moods: Developmental theory and measurement. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  75. Lewis, M., & Ramsay, D. (2005). Infant emotional and cortisol responses to goal blockage. Child Development, 76, 518–530.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  76. Lewis, M., Ramsay, D., & Sullivan, M. W. (2006). The relation of ans and hpa activation to infant anger and sadness response to goal blockage. Developmental Psychobiology, 48, 397–405.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  77. Lewis, M., Sullivan, M. W., & Michalson, L. (1984). The cognitive-emotional fugue. In J. Kagan, C. E. Izard, & R. B. Zajonc (Eds.), Emotions, cognitions, and behavior (pp. 264–288). London: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  78. Lewis, M., Sullivan, M. W., Ramsay, D., & Alessandri, S. M. (1992). Individual differences in anger and sad expressions during extinction: Antecedents and consequences. Infant Behavior & Development, 15(4), 443–452.Google Scholar
  79. Liu, D., Diorio, J., Tannenbaum, B., Caldji, C., Freedman, F. D., Sharma, S., et al. (1997). Maternal care, hippocampal glucocorticoid receptors, and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal responses to stress. Science, 277, 1659–1662.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  80. Lundberg, U., Rasch, B., & Westermark, O. (1990). Familial similarity in Type A behavior and physiological measurements. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 31(1), 34–41.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  81. Lutkenhaus, P. (1984). Pleasure derived from mastery in 3-year-olds: Its function for persistence and the influence of maternal behavior. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 7, 343–358.Google Scholar
  82. MacTurk, R. B., & Morgan, G. A. (Eds.). (1995). Mastery motivation: Origins, conceptulatizations, and applications. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
  83. Malatesta, C. Z., & Haviland, J. (1982). Learning display rules: The socialization of emotional expression in infancy. Child Development, 53, 991–1003.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  84. Marquis, D. (1943). A study of frustration in newborn infants. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 23, 123–138.Google Scholar
  85. Matthews, K. A., Woodall, K. L., Engebretson, T. O., & McCann, B. S. (1992). Influence of age, sex and family on type-A and hostile attitudes and behaviors. Health Psychology, 11(5), 317–323.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  86. Messer, D. (Ed.). (1993). Mastery motivation in early childhood: Development, measurement, and social processes. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  87. Moore, G. A., & Calkins, S. D. (2004). Infants’ vagal regulation in the still-face paradigm is related to dyadic coordination of mother-infant interaction. Developmental Psychology, 40(6), 1068–1080.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  88. Morrison, A. P. (Ed.). (1986). Essential papers on narcissism. New York: New York University Press.Google Scholar
  89. Nachimas, M., Gunnar, M. R., Mangelsdorf, S., Parritz, R. H., & Buss, K. (1996). Behavioral inhibition and stress reactivity: The moderating role of attachment security. Child Development, 67, 508–522.Google Scholar
  90. Nietzsche, F. ([1904]1964). Will to power. New York: Russell & Russell.Google Scholar
  91. Oginska-Bulik, N., & Juczynski, Z. (1998). Psychological correlates of the type A behavior pattern in children. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 29(4), 313–319.Google Scholar
  92. Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience. New York: Oxford Press.Google Scholar
  93. Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in children. New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
  94. Plutchik, R. (1980). A general psychoevolutionary theory of emotion. In R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (Eds.), Emotion: Theory, research, and experience (Vol. 1, pp. 3–33). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  95. Porges, S. W. (1998). Love and the evolution of the autonomic nervous system: The polyvagal theory of intimacy. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 23, 837–861.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  96. Porges, S. W. (2001). The polyvagal theory: Phylogenetic substrates of a social nervous system. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 42(2), 123–126.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  97. Porges, S. W., Doussard-Roosevelt, F. A., Portales, L., & Greenspan, S. (1996). Infant regulation of the vagal “brake” predicts child behavioral problems: A psychobiological model of social behavior. Developmental Psychology, 29, 697–712Google Scholar
  98. Potegal, M., Robison, S., Anderson, F., Jordan, C., & Shapiro, E. (2007) Sequence and priming in 15 month olds’ reactions to brief arm restraint: Evidence for a hierarchy of anger responses. Aggressive Behavior, 33, 1–11.Google Scholar
  99. Putnam, S. P., & Stifter, C. (2005). Behavioral approach-inhibition in toddlers: Prediction from infancy, positive and negative affective components, and relations with behavior problems. Child Development, 76(1), 212–226.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  100. Ramsay, D., King, S., & Lewis, M. (2005). The role of infant emotional response to goal blockage on toddler persistence. Poster presented at the SRCD Biennial International Conference, Atlanta, GA, April 7–10.Google Scholar
  101. Rank, O. (1945). Will therapy and truth and reality. New York: Knopf.Google Scholar
  102. Richards, J. E. (1995). Reliability of respiratory sinus arrhythmia in r-r intervals in 14-, 20-, and 26-week-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 18, 155–161.Google Scholar
  103. Rothbaum, F., Wolfer, J., & Visintainer, M. (1980). Coping behavior and locus of control in children. Journal of Personality, 47, 118–135.Google Scholar
  104. Rubin, K. H., Hastings, P. D., Stewart, S. L., Henderson, H. A., & Chen, X. (1997). The consistency and concomitants of inhibition: Some of the children, all of the time. Child Development, 68, 467–483.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  105. Sahar, T., Shalev, A. Y., & Porges, S. W. (2001). Vagal modulation of responses to mental challenge in post-traumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 47(7), 637–643.Google Scholar
  106. Sallis, J. F., Dimsdale, J. E., & Chipman, C. (1988). Invited review: Blood pressure reactivity in children. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 32(1), 1–12.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  107. Schmidt, L. A., Fox, N., Rubin, K. H., Sternberg, E. M., Gold, P. W., Smith, C. C., et al. (1997). Behavioral and neuroendocrine responses of shy children. Developmental Psychobiology, 30, 127–140.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  108. Schneirla, T. C. (1959). An evolutionary and developmental theory of biphasic processes underlying approach and withdrawal. In M. R. Jones (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 7, pp. 1–42). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
  109. Sears, R., & Sears, P. (1940). Minor studies of aggression, V: Strength of frustration-reaction as a function of strength of drive. Journal of Psychology, 9, 297–300.Google Scholar
  110. Seligman, M. E. P. (1975a). Helplessness: On depression, development and death. San Francisco: Freeman.Google Scholar
  111. Seligman, M. E. P. (1975b). On depression, development, and death. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
  112. Seligman, M. E. P. (1991). Learned optimism. New York: Knopf.Google Scholar
  113. Sherman, M., & Sherman, I. C. (1925). Sensorimotor responses in infants. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 5, 53–68.Google Scholar
  114. Sherman, M., Sherman, I. C., & Flory, C. D. (1936). Infant behavior. Comparative Psychology Monographs, 12, 1–107.Google Scholar
  115. Solomon, R. L. (1977). An opponent process theory of acquired motivation: The affective dynamics of addiction. In J. D. Maser & M. E. P. Seligman (Eds.), Psychopathology: Experimental models (pp. 66–103). San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
  116. Spangler, G., & Grossman, K. E. (1993). Behavioral organization in securely and insecurely attached infants. Child Development, 64, 1439–1450.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  117. Spangler, G., Schieche, M., Lllg, U., Maier, U., & Ackerman, C. (1994). Maternal sensitivity as an external organizer for biobehavioral regulation in infancy. Developmental Psychobiology, 27, 425–437.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  118. Sroufe, L. A. (1996). Emotional development: The organization of emotional life in the early years. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  119. Stein, N. L., & Jewett, J. L. (1986). A conceptual analysis of the meaning of negative emotions: Implications for a theory of development. In C. E. Izard & P. B. Read (Eds.), Measuring emotions in infants and children (Vol. 2, pp. 238–267). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  120. Stenberg, C. R. (1982). The development of anger facial expressions in infancy. Doctoral dissertation, University of Denver, Colorado.Google Scholar
  121. Stenberg, C. R., & Campos, J. J. (1990). The development of anger expressions in infancy. In N. L. Stein, B. Levanthal, & T. Trabasso (Eds.), Psychological and psychobiological approaches to emotion (pp. 247–267). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  122. Stenberg, C. R., Campos, J. J., & Emde, R. N. (1983). The facial expression of anger in seven-month-old infants. Child Development, 54, 178–184.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  123. Suarez, E. C., Williams, R. B., Kuhn, C. M., Zimmerman, E., & Schanberg, S. M. (1991). Biobehavioral basis of coronary-prone behavior in middle-aged men, part ii: Serum cholesterol, type-a behavior pattern, and hostility as interactive modulators of physiological reactivity. Psychosomatic Medicine, 53, 528–537.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  124. Sullivan, M. W. (2007). Individual differences in how babies become frustrated toddlers. Paper presented at the Institute for the Study of Child Development Brown Bag Series, UMDNJ-Robert Wood Johnson Medical School, New Brunswick, NJ, October 25, 2007.Google Scholar
  125. Sullivan, M. W., & Lewis, M. (2003). Contextual determinants of anger and other negative expressions in young infants. Developmental Psychology, 39(4), 693–705.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  126. Sullivan, M. W., Lewis, M., & Alessandri, S. M. (1992). Cross-age stability in emotional expressions during learning and extinction. Developmental Psychology, 28(1), 58–63.Google Scholar
  127. Suomi, S. J., Mineka, S., & Harlow, H. H. (1983). Social separation in monkeys as viewed from several motivational perspectives. In E. Satinoff & P. Titelbaum (Eds.), Handbook of neurobiology: Motivation (Vol. 6). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
  128. Tronick, E. (1989). Emotions and emotional communication in infants. American Psychologist, 44, 112–119.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  129. Turner, L. A., & Johnson, M. C. (2003). A model of mastery motivation for at-risk preschoolers. Educational Psychology, 95, 495–505.Google Scholar
  130. Wacker, J., Heldmann, M., & Stemmler, G. (2003). Separating emotion and motivational direction in fear and anger: Effects on frontal asymmetry. Emotion, 3(2), 167–193.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  131. Waldstein, S. R., Kop, W. J., Schmidt, L. A., Haufler, A. J., Krantz, D. S., & Fox, N. (2000). Frontal electrocortical and cardiovascular reactivity during happiness and anger. Biological Psychology, 55(3–23).PubMedGoogle Scholar
  132. Watson, J. B. (1925). Experimental studies on the growth of the emotions. Pediatric Seminars, 32, 328–348.Google Scholar
  133. Watson, J. S. (1972). Smiling, cooing, and the game. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 18, 323–339.Google Scholar
  134. White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of competence. Psychological Review, 66, 297–323.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  135. van Bakel, H. J., & Riksen-Walraven, J. M. (2004). Stress reactivity in 15-month old infants: Links with infant temperament, cognitive competence, and attachment security. Developmental Psychobiology, 44(3), 157–167.PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2010

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.University of Medicine and Dentistry of New JerseyNewarkUSA

Personalised recommendations