Moral Sensitivity in Young Gifted Children



This chapter explores the development of moral sensitivity from infancy through elementary years, using an empathy-based model. Moral sensitivity is the ability to act in a way that takes into account others' feelings and needs. Moral sensitivity requires awareness of another's suffering, and the desire to do something to alleviate that suffering. How gifted children might develop empathy within this model is explored. The effects of intensity and sensitivity are discussed within a framework of the asynchrony between what young gifted children feel and what they are able to do. Anecdotes, as well as preliminary research on empathy in gifted children, complement the descriptions of how gifted children struggle with issues related to moral sensitivity.


Moral Reasoning Moral Development Gifted Child Violent Video Game Moral Sensitivity 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.


  1. Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1964). Patterns of attachment behavior shown by the infant in interaction with his mother. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 10, 51–58.Google Scholar
  2. Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1969). Object relations, dependency and attachment: A theoretical review of the infant-mother relationship. Child Development, 40, 969–1026.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Blakemore, S. J. & Frith, U. (2003). How does the brain deal with the social world? NeuroReport, 14, 1–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bryant, B. K. (1982). An index of empathy for children and adolescents. Child Development, 53, 413–425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Calero, M. D., Garcia-Martin, M. B., Jimenez, M. I., Kazen, M., & Araque, A. (2007). Self-regulation advantage for high-IQ children: Findings from a research study. Learning and Individual Differences, 17, 328–343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Chan, D. W. (2007). Leadership competencies among Chinese gifted students in Hong Kong: The connection with emotional intelligence and successful intelligence. Roeper Review, 29, 183–189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Eisenberg, N., McCreath, H., & Ahn, R. (1988). Vicarious emotional responsiveness and prosocial behavior: Their interrelations in young children. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 14, 298–311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Murphy, B., Karbon, M., Maszk, P., Smith, M., O'Boyle, C., & Suh, K. (1994). The relations of emotionality and regulation to dispositional and situational empathy-related responding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 776–797.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Murphy, B., Karbon, M., Smith, M., & Maszk, P. (1996). The relations of children's dispositional empathy-related responding to their emotionality, regulation, and social functioning. Developmental Psychology, 32, 195–209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Eisenberg, N., Guthrie, I. K., Murphy, B. C., Shepard, S. A., Cumberland, A., & Carlo, G. (1999). Consistency and development of prosocial dispositions: A longitudinal study. Child Development, 70, 1360–1372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., & Sadovsky, A. (2006). Empathy-related responding in children. In M. Killen & J. G. Smetana (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 517–549). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  12. Funk, J. B., Buchman, D. D., Jenks, J., & Bechtoldt, H. (2003). Playing violent video games, desensitization, and moral evaluation in children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 24, 413–436.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Funk, J. B., Baldacci, H. B., Pasold, T., & Baumgardner, J. (2004). Violence exposure in real life, video games, television, movies and the Internet: Is there desensitization? Journal of Adolescence, 27, 23–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Gallese, V. (2003). The roots of empathy: The shared manifold hypothesis and the neural basis of intersubjectivity. Psychpathology, 36 (4), 171–180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Gopnik, A., Capps, L., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2000). Early theories of mind: What the theory can tell us about autism. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, & D. J. Cohen (Eds.),Understanding other minds (2nd Ed.) (pp. 50–72). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  16. Harris, J. C. (2003). Social neuroscience, empathy, brain integration, and neurodevelopmental disorders. Physiology and Behavior, 79, 525–531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Hoffman, M. L. (1994). Empathy, role taking, guilt, and development of altruistic motives. In B. Puka (Ed.), Reaching out: Caring, altruism and prosocial behavior (pp. 196–218). New York: Garland.Google Scholar
  18. Hoffman, M. L. (2000). Empathy and moral development: Implications for caring and justice. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  19. Iacoboni, M., Woods, R. P., Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Mazziotta, J. C., & Rizzolatti, G. (1999). Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. Science, 286 (5449), 2526–2528.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Jefferson, B. M. (2007, March 15). Their hearts are with the homeless. The Providence Journal. Available:–15–07.Jefferson, B. M. (2007, March 15). Their hearts are with the homeless. The Providence Journal. Available:–15–07.
  21. Kohlberg, L. (1984). The psychology of moral development. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
  22. Lovecky, D. V. (1994). The moral gifted child in a violent world. Understanding Our Gifted, 6, 1, 3.Google Scholar
  23. Lovecky, D. V. (1997). Identity development in gifted children: Moral sensitivity. Roeper Review, 20, 90–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Lovecky, D. V. (2004). Different minds: Gifted children with AD/HD, Asperger Syndrome and other learning deficits. New York: Jessica Kingsley.Google Scholar
  25. Mehrabian, A. & Epstein, N. A. (1972). A measure of emotional empathy. Journal of Personality, 40, 237–247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Milligan, K., Astington, J. W., & Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Development, 78, 622–646.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Perner, J., Leekam, S. R., & Wimmer, H. (1987). Three-year-olds' difficulty with false belief: The case for a conceptual deficit. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 125–137.Google Scholar
  28. Piaget, J. (1965). The moral development of the child (M. Gabain, Trans.). New York: Free Press. (Original work published 1932)Google Scholar
  29. Piaget, J. & Inhelder, B. (1969). The psychology of the child. (H. Weaver, Trans.). New York: BasicBooks. (Original work published 1966)Google Scholar
  30. Piechowski, M. M. (2006). Mellow out, they say. If only I could: Intensities and sensitivities of the young and bright. Madison, WI: Yunasa Books.Google Scholar
  31. Rest, J. (1979). Development in judging moral issues. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
  32. Roeper, A. (1982). How the gifted cope with their emotions. Roeper Review, 5 (2), 21–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Roeper, A. (1995). Finding the clue to children's thought processes. In R. C. Medeiros, L. K. Silverman, & P. Espeland (Eds.), Annemarie Roeper: Selected speeches and writings (pp. 22–38). Minneapolis, MN: Free Spirit.Google Scholar
  34. Sagi, A. & Hoffman, M. L. (1976). Empathic distress in the newborn. Developmental Psychology, 12, 175–176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Siegel, D. J. (1999). The developing mind: How relationships and the brain interact to shape who we are. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
  36. Silverman, L. K. (1993a). The gifted individual. In L. K. Silverman (Ed.), Counseling the gifted and talented (pp. 3–28). Denver: Love.Google Scholar
  37. Silverman, L. K. (1993b). Social development, leadership, and gender issues. In L. K. Silverman (Ed.), Counseling the gifted and talented (pp. 291–327). Denver: Love.Google Scholar
  38. Simner, M. L. (1971). Newborn's response to the cry of another infant. Developmental Psychology, 5, 136–150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Stern, D. N. (1985/2000). The interpersonal world of the infant (First paperback edition). New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  40. Thomas, A., Chess, S., & Birch, S. G. (1968). Temperament and behavior disorders in children. New York: New York University Press.Google Scholar
  41. Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72, 655–684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.ProvidenceUSA

Personalised recommendations