Psychobiological functions of the mammary pheromone in newborn rabbits

  • Gérard Coureaud
  • Benoist Schaal


Lactating female rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus) nurse once daily for 5 min. The pups are thus forced to localize the nipples quickly. The females emit multiple chemosignals to guide them, among which are common odors derived from dietary aromas and one identified pheromone. Thus, the mammary pheromone (MP) released in rabbit milk is highly behaviorally active: it is a potent releaser of suckling-related behaviors in pups. The MP also acts as a strong reinforcer for early odor learning. Both of these functions of the MP are active right after birth and are thus in a position to play a central role in neonatal adaptation. However, the releasing function and the reinforcing functions of the MP follow different time-courses, suggesting that they are controlled by different underlying mechanisms. These data illustrate how a same chemical signal can carry distinct functions in a newborn mammal.


Conditioned Stimulus Behavioral Activity Oryctolagus Cuniculus Young Rabbit Newborn Rabbit 


Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.


  1. Bautista, A., Mendoza-Degante, M., Coureaud, G., Martina-Gomez, M., and Hudson, R. (2005) Scramble competition in newborn domestic rabbits for an unusually restricted milk supply. Anim. Behav. 70, 997–1002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Beauchamp, G.K., Doty, R.L., Moulton, D.G. and Mugford, R.A. (1976) The pheromone concept in mammalian chemical communication. In Doty R.L. (Ed.), Mammalian Olfaction, Reproductive Processes and Behaviour. Academic Press, New York, pp 143–160.Google Scholar
  3. Bilkó, A., Altbäcker, V. and Hudson, R. (1994) Transmission of food preference in the rabbit: The means of information transfer. Physiol. Behav. 56, 907–912.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bronson, F.H. (1971) Rodent pheromones. Biol. Reprod. 4, 344–357.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Coureaud, G. (2001) Olfactory regulation of sucking in newborn rabbit: Ethological and chemical characterization of a pheromonal signal. PhD Thesis, University of Paris.Google Scholar
  6. Coureaud, G. and Schaal, B. (2000) Attraction of newborn rabbits to abdominal odors of adult conspecifics differing in sex and physiological state. Dev. Psychobiol. 36, 271–281.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Coureaud, G., Langlois, D., Perrier, G. and Schaal B. (2003) A single key-odorant accounts for the pheromonal effect of rabbit milk: Further test of the mammary pheromone’s activity against a wide sample of volatiles from milk. Chemoecology 13, 187–192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Coureaud, G., Langlois, D., Perrier, G. and Schaal, B. (2006a). Convergent changes in the maternal emission and pup reception of the rabbit mammary pheromone. Chemoecology 16, 169–174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Coureaud, G., Langlois, D., Sicard, G. and Schaal, B. (2004) Newborn rabbit reactivity to the mammary pheromone: Concentration-response relationship. Chem. Senses 29, 341–350.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Coureaud, G., Montigny, D., Dewas, M., Moncomble, A.S., Patris, P. and Schaal, S. (2005) A sensitive period for pheromone-induced odour learning in newborn rabbits. 29th International Ethological Conference, Budapest, Hungary, 20–27 August.Google Scholar
  11. Coureaud, G., Moncomble, A.S., Montigny, D., Dewas, M., Perrier, G. and Schaal, B. (2006c) A pheromone that rapidly promotes learning in the newborn. Curr. Biol. 16, 1956–1961.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Coureaud, G., Rodel, H.G., Kurz, C., von Holst, D. and Schaal, B. (2006b) Age-related changes in the processing of the rabbit mammary pheromone: comparison in domestic and wild pups. 17th Congress of the European Chemoreception Research Organisation, Granada, Spain, and Chem. Senses 31, 274 (abstract online).Google Scholar
  13. Coureaud, G., Schaal, B., Coudert, P., Rideaud, P., Fortun-Lamothe, L., Hudson, R. and Orgeur, P. (2000) Immediate postnatal sucking in the rabbit: its influence on pup survival and growth. Reprod. Nutr. Dev. 40, 19–32.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Coureaud, G., Schaal, B., Hudson, R., Orgeur, P. and Coudert, P. (2002) Transnatal olfactory continuity in the rabbit: behavioral evidence and short-term consequence of its disruption. Dev. Psychobiol. 40, 372–390.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Coureaud, G., Schaal, B., Langlois, D. and Perrier G (2001) Orientation response of newborn rabbits to odours emitted by lactating females: Relative effectiveness of surface and milk cues. Anim. Behav. 61, 153–162.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Distel, H. and Hudson, R. (1985) The contribution of olfactory and tactile modalities to the performance of nipple-search behaviour in newborn rabbits. J. Comp. Physiol. A 157, 599–605.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Drummond, H. Vázquez, E., Sanchez-Colón, S., Martinez-Gómez, M. and Hudson, R. (2000) Competition for milk in the domestic rabbit: Survivors benefit from littermate deaths. Ethology 106, 511–526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. González-Mariscal, G., Chirino, R. and Hudson, R. (1994) Prolactin stimulates emission of nipple pheromone in ovariectomized New Zealand white rabbits. Biol. Reprod. 50, 373–376.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Hudson, R. and Distel H. (1983) Nipple location by newborn rabbits: behavioural evidence for pheromonal guidance. Behaviour 85, 260–275.Google Scholar
  20. Hudson, R. and Distel, H. (1984) Nipple-search pheromone in rabbits: dependence on season and reproductive state. J. Comp. Physiol. A 155, 13–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Hudson, R. and Distel, H. (1986) Pheromonal release of suckling in rabbits does not depend on the vomeronasal organ. Physiol. Behav. 37, 123–129.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Jilge, B. and Hudson, R. (2001) Diversity and development of the circadian rhythms in the European rabbit. Chronobiol. Int. 18, 1–26.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Johnston, R.E. (2000) Chemical communication and pheromones: The types of chemical signals and the role of the vomeronasal system. In Finger T.E., Silver W.L., and Restrepo D. (Eds.), The Neurobiology of Taste and Smell. Wiley-Liss, New York, pp 101–127.Google Scholar
  24. Karlson, P. and Lø‘scher, M. (1959) ’’Pheromones”: a new term for a class of biologically active substances. Nature 183, 55–56.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Keil, W., von Stralendorff, F. and Hudson, R. (1990) A behavioral bioassay for analysis of rabbit nipple-search pheromone. Physiol. Behav. 47, 525–529.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Kindermann, U., Gervais, R. and Hudson, R. (1991) Rapid odor conditioning in newborn rabbits: Amnesic effect of hypothermia. Physiol. Behav. 50, 457–460.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Moncomble, A.S., Coureaud, G., Quennedey, B., Langlois, D., Perrier, G., Brossut, R. and Schaal, B. (2005) The mammary pheromone of the rabbit: where does it come from? Anim. Behav. 69, 29–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Montigny, D., Coureaud, G. and Schaal, B. (2005) Circadian and developmental fluctuations in the releasing and reinforcing potencies of the rabbit mammary pheromone. 16th Congress of the European Chemoreception Research Organisation, Dijon, France, and Chem. Senses 30(2), 180 (abstract online).Google Scholar
  29. Montigny, D., Coureaud, G. and Schaal, B. (2006) Newborn rabbit response to the mammary pheromone: From automatism to prandial control. Physiol. Behav. 89, 742–749.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Müller, K. (1978) Zum Saugverhalten von Kaninchen unter besonderer Berücksichtigung des Geruchsvermögens. PhD Thesis, University of Giessen.Google Scholar
  31. Nordlund, D.A. and Lewis W.J. (1976) Terminology of chemical releasing stimuli in intraspecific and interspecific interactions. J. Chem. Ecol. 2, 211–220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Rosenblatt, J.S. (1983) Olfaction mediates developmental transition in the altricial newborn of selected species of mammals. Dev. Psychobiol. 16, 347–375.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Schaal, B., Coureaud, G., Langlois, D., Giniès, C., Sémon, E. and Perrier, G. (2003) Chemical and behavioural characterization of the rabbit mammary pheromone. Nature 424, 68–72.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Schaal, B., Coureaud, G., Marlier, L. and Soussignan, R. (2001) Fetal olfactory cognition preadapts neonatal behavior in mammals. In Marchlewska-Koj A., Lepri J. and Müller-Schwarze D. (Eds.), Chemical Signals in Vertebrates, Volume 9. Plenum-Klüwer Academic, New York, pp 197–205.Google Scholar
  35. Schley, P. (1981) Olfaction and suckling in young rabbits. Kleintierpraxis 26, 261–263.Google Scholar
  36. Semke E., Distel H. and Hudson, R. (1995) Specific enhancement of olfactory receptor sensitivity associated with foetal learning of food odours in the rabbit. Naturwissenschaften 82, 148–149.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Sullivan, R.M., Hofer, M.A. and Brake, S.C. (1986) Olfactory-guided orientation in neonatal rats is enhanced by a conditioned change in behavioral state. Dev. Psychobiol. 19, 615–623.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Wilson, E.O. and Bossert, W.H. (1963) Chemical communication among animals. Recent Prog. Horm. Res. 19, 673–716.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Zarrow, M. X., Denenberg, V. H. and Anderson, C. O. (1965) Rabbit: frequency of suckling in the pup. Science 150, 1835–1836.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media,LLC 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  • Gérard Coureaud
    • 1
  • Benoist Schaal
  1. 1.Centre Européen des Sciences du GoÛtCNRS/Univ.Bourgogne/Inra15 rue Hugues PicardetFrance

Personalised recommendations