Advertisement

Telephone conferences for fun: experimentation in people’s homes

  • Andrew F. Monk
  • Darren J. Reed
Part of the IFIP — The International Federation for Information Processing book series (IFIPAICT, volume 241)

Abstract

The paper illustrates how communication experiments may be carried out in a domestic recreational context. Participants situated in their homes were connected into group telephone conversations and simply asked to “chat and enjoy yourselves”. Following the conversation, participants provided Likert scale ratings of the experience. In addition, the conversations were recorded and analysed. A total of 211 participants took part in two experiments. Telephone groups had an average size of five people, each speaking in the same conversation from their individual homes. Comments from the participants and Likert rating scales indicated that it was a positive recreational experience. The primary manipulation in each experiment was intended to encourage spontaneous co-involvement of all of the group. In Experiment 1 this was done by changing the way participants were introduced to the group, in Experiment 2 the group was made more salient by providing each member with a list of names. Open ended responses obtained from telephone interviews with participants in Experiment 1 were used to construct a questionnaire for Experiment 2 to measure presence, involvement and communication efficacy. The measures extracted from transcripts included the average length of utterances and equality of contribution as well as a new measure, the number of lines to the first “flow episode” in the transcript. While neither of the manipulations produced significant effects on Likert scale ratings made after the sessions or the measures extracted from transcripts, the paper is able to recommend the measures used and to provide practical advice for other investigators seeking to run communication experiments in a recreational context.

Keywords

Group Size List Condition Conversational Analysis Likert Scale Rating Primary Manipulation 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. 1.
    Reed, D.J., Fun on the phone: the situated experience of recreational telephone conferences, in Funology: from usability to enjoyment, M.A. Blythe, et al., Editors., Kluwer: Dordrecht, the Netherlands (2003) 67–79Google Scholar
  2. 2.
    Reed, D.J. and A.F. Monk. Using familiar technologies in unfamiliar ways and learning from the old about the new. Universal Access in the Information Society, 3 (2004) 114–121CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. 3.
    Short, J., F.. Williams, and B. Christie, The social psychology of telecommunications, London: John Wiley and Sons (1976)Google Scholar
  4. 4.
    Chapanis, A.. Interactive Human Communication. Scientific American, 232 (1975, March) 36–42Google Scholar
  5. 5.
    Finn, K.E., A.J. Sellen, and S.B. Wilbur, Video-mediated communication, Mahwah. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates (1997)Google Scholar
  6. 6.
    Anderson, A.H., J. Mullin, R. Katsavras, R. McEwan. E. Grattan, P. Brundell, and C. O’Malley. Multimediating multiparty interactions, in Interact’99. Edinburgh: Amsterdam: IOS Press (1999)Google Scholar
  7. 7.
    Monk, A.F. and L.A. Watts, Peripheral participation in video-mediated communication. International Journal of Human-computer Studies.. 52 (2000) 775–960CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. 8.
    Monk, A.F., E. Fellas, and E. Ley, Hearing only one side of normal and mobile phone conversations. Behaviour and Information Technology, 23 (2004) 301–305CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. 9.
    Daly-Jones, O., A.F. Monk, and L.A. Watts. Some advantages of video conferencing over high-quality audio conferencing: fluency and awareness of attentional focus. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 1998, 49(1): p. 21–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. 10.
    Carletta, J., S. Garrod, and H. Fraser-Krauss, Communication and Placement of Authority in Workplace Groups: The Consequences for Innovation. Small Group Research, 1998. 29(5): p. 531–559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. 11.
    Fay, N., S. Garrod, and J. Carletta, Group discussion as interactive dialogue or as serial monologue: the influence of group size. Psychological Science, 2000, 11(6): p. 481–486.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. 12.
    Goffman, E., Frame analysis, an essay on the organisation of experience, 1974, Boston: North East University Press.Google Scholar
  13. 13.
    Goffman, E., Fun in games, in Encounters: two studies in the sociology of interaction, E. Goffman, Editor. 1961, Bob Merril: Indianapolis.Google Scholar
  14. 14.
    Edelsky, C, Who’s got the floor. Language in Society, 1981. 10: p. 383–421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. 15.
    Csikszentmihalyi, M. and K. Rathunde, The measurement of flow in everyday life: towards a theory of emergent motivation. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1993. 40: p. 57–97.Google Scholar
  16. 16.
    Strauss, A. and J. Corbin, Basics of qualitative research: grounded theory procedures and techniques. 1990, Newbury Park: Sage.Google Scholar
  17. 17.
    Lindley, S.E. Designing interfaces to afford enjoyable social interactions by collocated groups. Proceedings of CHI 2005, Extended Abstracts, 2005, 1122–1123. New York: ACM Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© International Federation for Information Processing 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  • Andrew F. Monk
    • 1
  • Darren J. Reed
    • 2
  1. 1.Centre for Usable Home Technology (CUHTec)University of YorkUK
  2. 2.Department of SociologyUniversity of YorkUK

Personalised recommendations