Skip to main content

Part of the book series: Springer International Handbooks of Education ((SIHE,volume 21))

Recent international studies of reading have expanded our understanding of how writing systems (orthographies) map onto spoken language (phonology) and the processes by which understanding of written language occurs (Rayner, Foorman, Perfetti, Pesetsky, & Seidenberg, 2001; Snowling & Hulme, 2005). From an international perspective, the teaching of reading is first and foremost a matter of grain size — the size of the orthographic unit that maps to the relevant phonological unit. Ziegler and Goswami (2005) argue that in English alphabetic letters map to multiple phonological units — whole words, onsets and rimes, and phonemes, and that, consequently, learning to read in English is more complex than learning to read in languages with a match in grain size, such as Finnish, Italian, Spanish, German, and Greek (Seymour, Aro, & Erskine, 2003).

In the United States, the teaching of reading has become a matter of public policy with the passage of the No Child Left Behind Act of 2001 (NCLB; Public Law No. 107–110) and its provisions for closing the achievement gap between advantaged and disadvantaged children by holding schools accountable for achievement results and requiring that teachers be highly qualified (Foorman, Kalinowski, & Sexton, 2007). The Reading First component of NCLB targets beginning reading instruction and is based on consensus documents summarizing over 30 years of research (National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 2000; National Research Council, 1998; RAND Reading Study Group, 2002). In this chapter we have two broad objectives to (a) explain what is known scientifically about learning to read English, and (b) summarize research on reading instruction.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 749.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 949.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 949.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chall, J. S. (1967). Learning to read: The great debate. New York: McGraw Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N., & Halle, M. (1968). The sound pattern of English. NY: Harper and Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clay, M. (1993). Reading recovery: A guidebook for teachers in training. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clymer, T. (1963). The utility of phonic generalization's in the primary grades. Reading Teacher, 16, 252–258.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dolch, E. W. (1953). The Dolch basic sight word list. Champaign, IL: Garrard.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foorman, B. R. (1986). Non-alphabetic codes in learning to read: The case of the Japanese. In B. Foorman & A. Siegel (Eds.), Acquisition of reading skills: Cultural constraints and cognitive universals (pp. 115–122). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foorman, B. R., & Ciancio, D. (2005). Screening for secondary intervention: Concept and context. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38(6), 494–499.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foorman, B. R., & Connor, C. M. (in press). Primary reading. In M. Kamil, P. D. Pearson, E. B. Moje, & P. Afflerbach (Eds.), Handbook of Reading Research (Vol. 4).

    Google Scholar 

  • Foorman, B. R., & Torgesen, J. K. (2001). Critical elements of classroom and small-group instruction promote reading success in all children. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 16(4), 203–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foorman, B. R., Chen, D. T., Carlson, C., Moats, L. C., Francis, D. J., & Fletcher, J. M. (2003). The necessity of the alphabetic principle to phonemic awareness instruction. Reading and Writing, 16, 289–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foorman, B. R., Francis, D. J., Davidson, K., Harm, M., & Griffin, J. (2004). Variability in text features in six grade 1 basal reading programs. Scientific Studies of Reading, 8(2), 167–197.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foorman, B. R., Francis, D. J., Fletcher, J. M., Schatschneider, C., & Mehta, P. (1998). The role of instruction in learning to read: Preventing reading failure in at-risk children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 37–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foorman, B. R., Kalinowski, S. J., & Sexton, W. L. (2007). Standards-based educational reform is one important step toward reducing the achievement gap. In A. Gamoran (Ed.), Standards-based reform and the poverty gap: Lessons from “No Child Left Behind”. Washington, DC: Brookings Institution.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foorman, B. R., Schatschneider, C., Eakin, M. N., Fletcher, J. M., Moats, L. C., & Francis, D. J. (2006). The impact of instructional practices in grades 1 and 2 on reading and spelling achievement in high poverty schools. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 31, 1–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. J., & Levin, H. (1975). The psychology of reading. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, K. S. (1970). Reading: A psycholinguistic guessing game. In H. Singer & R. Ruddell (Eds.), Theoretical models and processes of reading (pp. 259–272). Newark, NJ: International Reading Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, K. S., & Burke, C. L. (1973). Theoretically based studies of patterns of miscues in oral reading performance (U.S. Office of Education Project No. 9-0375). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodman, K. S., & Goodman, Y. M. (1979). Learning to read is natural. In L. Resnick & P. Weaver (Eds.), Theory and practice of early reading (Vol. 1). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U. (1998). The role of analogies in the development of word recognition. In J. Metsala & L. C. Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. B., & Hillinger, M. L. (1980). Learning to read: An unnatural act. Bulletin of the Orton Society, 30, 179–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. B., Juel, C., & Griffith, P. L. (1992). Reading, spelling, and the orthographic cipher. In P. B. Gough, L. C. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 35–48). Hillsdale, NY: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7, 6–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hanna, P. R., Hanna, J. S., Hodges, R. E., & Rudorf, E. H. (1966). Phoneme-grapheme correspondences as cues to spelling improvement. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Juel, C., & Minden-Cupp, C. (2000). Learning to read words: Linguistic units and instructional strategies. Reading Research Quarterly, 35, 458–492.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Juel, C., & Roper-Schneider, D. (1985). The influence of basal readers on first-grade reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 18, 306–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mathes, P. G., Denton, C. A., Fletcher, J. M., Anthony, J. L., Francis, D. J., & Schatashneider, C. (2005). The effects of theoretically different instruction and student characteristics on the skills of struggling readers. Reading Research Quarterly, 40(2), 148–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mehta, P., Foorman, B. R., Branum-Martin, L., & Taylor, W. P. (2005). Literacy as a unidimensional multilevel construct: Validation, sources of influence, and implications in a longitudinal study in grades 1–4. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9(2), 85–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moats, L. C. (2000). Speech to Print. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. (2000). National Reading Panel — Teaching children to read: Reports of the subgroups (NIH Publication No. 00-4754). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • National Research Council. (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children. In C. E. Snow, M. S. Burns, & P. Griffin (Eds.), Committee on the Prevention of Reading Difficulties in Young Children, Committee on Behavioral and Social Science and Education. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • No Child Left Behind Act (2001). Public Law No. 107–110, 115 Stat. 1425.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A. (1985). Reading ability. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • RAND Reading Study Group. (2002). Reading for understanding. Santa Monica, CA: RAND.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., Foorman, B. R., Perfetti, C. A., Pesetsky, D., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2001). How psychological science informs the teaching of reading. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 2, 31–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 143–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55, 151–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slavin, R. E., Madden, N. A., Dolan, L. J., & Wasik, B. A. (1996). Every child, every school: Success for all. Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, F. (1973). Psycholinguistics and reading. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, F., & Goodman, K. S. (1971). On the psycholinguistic method of teaching reading. Elementary School Journal, 71, 177–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. J. & Hulme, C. (Eds.). (2005). The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 135–154). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1980). Towards an interactive-compensatory model of individual differences in the development of reading fluency. Reading Research Quarterly, 16, 32–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stuebing, K. K., Barth, A., Cirino, P., Fletcher, J. M., & Francis, D. J. (2008). A response to recent re-analyses of The National Reading Panel Report: Effects of systematic phonics instruction are practically significant. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 123–134

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R. (1993). Beginning to spell. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2009 Springer Science+Business Media, LLC

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Foorman, B.R., Santi, K.L. (2009). The Teaching of Reading. In: Saha, L.J., Dworkin, A.G. (eds) International Handbook of Research on Teachers and Teaching. Springer International Handbooks of Education, vol 21. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-73317-3_62

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics