Advertisement

The Teaching of Reading

  • Barbara R. Foorman
  • Kristi L. Santi
Part of the Springer International Handbooks of Education book series (SIHE, volume 21)

Recent international studies of reading have expanded our understanding of how writing systems (orthographies) map onto spoken language (phonology) and the processes by which understanding of written language occurs (Rayner, Foorman, Perfetti, Pesetsky, & Seidenberg, 2001; Snowling & Hulme, 2005). From an international perspective, the teaching of reading is first and foremost a matter of grain size — the size of the orthographic unit that maps to the relevant phonological unit. Ziegler and Goswami (2005) argue that in English alphabetic letters map to multiple phonological units — whole words, onsets and rimes, and phonemes, and that, consequently, learning to read in English is more complex than learning to read in languages with a match in grain size, such as Finnish, Italian, Spanish, German, and Greek (Seymour, Aro, & Erskine, 2003).

In the United States, the teaching of reading has become a matter of public policy with the passage of the No Child Left Behind Act of 2001 (NCLB; Public Law No. 107–110) and its provisions for closing the achievement gap between advantaged and disadvantaged children by holding schools accountable for achievement results and requiring that teachers be highly qualified (Foorman, Kalinowski, & Sexton, 2007). The Reading First component of NCLB targets beginning reading instruction and is based on consensus documents summarizing over 30 years of research (National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, 2000; National Research Council, 1998; RAND Reading Study Group, 2002). In this chapter we have two broad objectives to (a) explain what is known scientifically about learning to read English, and (b) summarize research on reading instruction.

Keywords

Reading Comprehension Phonemic Awareness Oral Reading Oral Reading Fluency English Orthography 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  2. Chall, J. S. (1967). Learning to read: The great debate. New York: McGraw Hill.Google Scholar
  3. Chomsky, N., & Halle, M. (1968). The sound pattern of English. NY: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
  4. Clay, M. (1993). Reading recovery: A guidebook for teachers in training. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.Google Scholar
  5. Clymer, T. (1963). The utility of phonic generalization's in the primary grades. Reading Teacher, 16, 252–258.Google Scholar
  6. Dolch, E. W. (1953). The Dolch basic sight word list. Champaign, IL: Garrard.Google Scholar
  7. Foorman, B. R. (1986). Non-alphabetic codes in learning to read: The case of the Japanese. In B. Foorman & A. Siegel (Eds.), Acquisition of reading skills: Cultural constraints and cognitive universals (pp. 115–122). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  8. Foorman, B. R., & Ciancio, D. (2005). Screening for secondary intervention: Concept and context. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38(6), 494–499.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Foorman, B. R., & Connor, C. M. (in press). Primary reading. In M. Kamil, P. D. Pearson, E. B. Moje, & P. Afflerbach (Eds.), Handbook of Reading Research (Vol. 4).Google Scholar
  10. Foorman, B. R., & Torgesen, J. K. (2001). Critical elements of classroom and small-group instruction promote reading success in all children. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 16(4), 203–212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Foorman, B. R., Chen, D. T., Carlson, C., Moats, L. C., Francis, D. J., & Fletcher, J. M. (2003). The necessity of the alphabetic principle to phonemic awareness instruction. Reading and Writing, 16, 289–324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Foorman, B. R., Francis, D. J., Davidson, K., Harm, M., & Griffin, J. (2004). Variability in text features in six grade 1 basal reading programs. Scientific Studies of Reading, 8(2), 167–197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Foorman, B. R., Francis, D. J., Fletcher, J. M., Schatschneider, C., & Mehta, P. (1998). The role of instruction in learning to read: Preventing reading failure in at-risk children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 37–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Foorman, B. R., Kalinowski, S. J., & Sexton, W. L. (2007). Standards-based educational reform is one important step toward reducing the achievement gap. In A. Gamoran (Ed.), Standards-based reform and the poverty gap: Lessons from “No Child Left Behind”. Washington, DC: Brookings Institution.Google Scholar
  15. Foorman, B. R., Schatschneider, C., Eakin, M. N., Fletcher, J. M., Moats, L. C., & Francis, D. J. (2006). The impact of instructional practices in grades 1 and 2 on reading and spelling achievement in high poverty schools. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 31, 1–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Gibson, E. J., & Levin, H. (1975). The psychology of reading. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.Google Scholar
  17. Goodman, K. S. (1970). Reading: A psycholinguistic guessing game. In H. Singer & R. Ruddell (Eds.), Theoretical models and processes of reading (pp. 259–272). Newark, NJ: International Reading Association.Google Scholar
  18. Goodman, K. S., & Burke, C. L. (1973). Theoretically based studies of patterns of miscues in oral reading performance (U.S. Office of Education Project No. 9-0375). Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.Google Scholar
  19. Goodman, K. S., & Goodman, Y. M. (1979). Learning to read is natural. In L. Resnick & P. Weaver (Eds.), Theory and practice of early reading (Vol. 1). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  20. Goswami, U. (1998). The role of analogies in the development of word recognition. In J. Metsala & L. C. Ehri (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  21. Gough, P. B., & Hillinger, M. L. (1980). Learning to read: An unnatural act. Bulletin of the Orton Society, 30, 179–196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Gough, P. B., Juel, C., & Griffith, P. L. (1992). Reading, spelling, and the orthographic cipher. In P. B. Gough, L. C. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 35–48). Hillsdale, NY: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  23. Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7, 6–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hanna, P. R., Hanna, J. S., Hodges, R. E., & Rudorf, E. H. (1966). Phoneme-grapheme correspondences as cues to spelling improvement. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.Google Scholar
  25. Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
  26. Juel, C., & Minden-Cupp, C. (2000). Learning to read words: Linguistic units and instructional strategies. Reading Research Quarterly, 35, 458–492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Juel, C., & Roper-Schneider, D. (1985). The influence of basal readers on first-grade reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 18, 306–327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Mathes, P. G., Denton, C. A., Fletcher, J. M., Anthony, J. L., Francis, D. J., & Schatashneider, C. (2005). The effects of theoretically different instruction and student characteristics on the skills of struggling readers. Reading Research Quarterly, 40(2), 148–183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Mehta, P., Foorman, B. R., Branum-Martin, L., & Taylor, W. P. (2005). Literacy as a unidimensional multilevel construct: Validation, sources of influence, and implications in a longitudinal study in grades 1–4. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9(2), 85–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Moats, L. C. (2000). Speech to Print. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
  31. National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. (2000). National Reading Panel — Teaching children to read: Reports of the subgroups (NIH Publication No. 00-4754). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.Google Scholar
  32. National Research Council. (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children. In C. E. Snow, M. S. Burns, & P. Griffin (Eds.), Committee on the Prevention of Reading Difficulties in Young Children, Committee on Behavioral and Social Science and Education. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
  33. No Child Left Behind Act (2001). Public Law No. 107–110, 115 Stat. 1425.Google Scholar
  34. Perfetti, C. A. (1985). Reading ability. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  35. RAND Reading Study Group. (2002). Reading for understanding. Santa Monica, CA: RAND.Google Scholar
  36. Rayner, K., Foorman, B. R., Perfetti, C. A., Pesetsky, D., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2001). How psychological science informs the teaching of reading. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 2, 31–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 143–174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55, 151–218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Slavin, R. E., Madden, N. A., Dolan, L. J., & Wasik, B. A. (1996). Every child, every school: Success for all. Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin Press.Google Scholar
  40. Smith, F. (1973). Psycholinguistics and reading. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.Google Scholar
  41. Smith, F., & Goodman, K. S. (1971). On the psycholinguistic method of teaching reading. Elementary School Journal, 71, 177–181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Snowling, M. J. & Hulme, C. (Eds.). (2005). The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 135–154). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  43. Stanovich, K. E. (1980). Towards an interactive-compensatory model of individual differences in the development of reading fluency. Reading Research Quarterly, 16, 32–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Stuebing, K. K., Barth, A., Cirino, P., Fletcher, J. M., & Francis, D. J. (2008). A response to recent re-analyses of The National Reading Panel Report: Effects of systematic phonics instruction are practically significant. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 123–134CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Treiman, R. (1993). Beginning to spell. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  46. Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2009

Authors and Affiliations

  • Barbara R. Foorman
    • 1
  • Kristi L. Santi
    • 2
  1. 1.Florida Center for Reading ResearchFlorida State UniversityTallahassee32301
  2. 2.The Santi GroupHouston

Personalised recommendations