Skip to main content

Consciousness and Self-Directed Attention

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
The Neuropsychology of Attention

Abstract

The relationship between external reality and people’s subjective experience of it has been the subject of philosophical inquiry over the past 2,000 years. These efforts reached a peak during the European Renaissance in the debates between adherents of monism vs. dualism over whether or not objective reality and subjective experience are unified or entirely distinct phenomena that can never being fully reduced or understood relative to one another. A manifestation of this debate was the mind–body problem. Is consideration of the physical and mechanistic underpinnings of human physiology necessary for understanding mental experience? Furthermore, is objectivity theoretically possible if subjective mental experience is distinct from objective reality? Questions regarding the nature of human consciousness and awareness of self relative to the external world have tended to be either implicitly or explicitly embedded in these philosophical debates which continue even today.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 169.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  1. Pribram, K. H. (1976). Problems concerning the structure of consciousness. In G. Globus, G. Maxell, & I. Savodnik (Eds.), Science and the mind-brain puzzle. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Churchland, P. S., & Sejnowski, T. J. (1988). Perspectives on cognitive neuroscience. Science (New York, NY), 242(4879), 741–745.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Churchland, P. S., & Sejnowski, T. J. (1992). The computational brain. Cambridge: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Norton, L., Hutchison, R. M., Young, G. B., Lee, D. H., Sharpe, M. D., & Mirsattari, S. M. (2012). Disruptions of functional connectivity in the default mode network of comatose patients. Neurology, 78(3), 175–181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Crone, J. S., Ladurner, G., Holler, Y., Golaszewski, S., Trinka, E., & Kronbichler, M. (2011). Deactivation of the default mode network as a marker of impaired consciousness: An fMRI study. PLoS One, 6(10), e26373.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Searle, J. (1990). Is the brain’s mind a computer program? No. A program merely manipulates symbols, whereas a brain attaches meaning to them. Scientific American, 202, 26–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Daugman, J. (1990). Brain metaphor and brain theory. In E. L. Schwartz (Ed.), Computational neuroscience. Boston: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Power, J. D., Cohen, A. L., Nelson, S. M., et al. (2011). Functional network organization of the human brain. Neuron, 72(4), 665–678.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Dennett, D. C. (1996). Kinds of minds: Toward an understanding of consciousness. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Dennett, D. C., Ito, M., Miyashita, Y., & Rolls, E. T. (1997). Consciousness in human and robot minds. In M. Ito, Y. Miyashita, & E. T. Rolls (Eds.), Cognition, computation, and consciousness (pp. 17–29). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  11. Dennett, D. C., & Kinsbourne, M. (1992). Time and the observer: The where and when of consciousness in the brain. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 15(2), 183–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Dennett, D. C., & Levitin, D. J. (2002). Where am I? In D. J. Levitin (Ed.), Foundations of cognitive psychology: Core readings (pp. 23–33). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Dennett, D. C., & Weiner, P. (1991). Consciousness explained. Boston, MA: Little, Brown.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Brewer, J. A., Worhunsky, P. D., Gray, J. R., Tang, Y. Y., Weber, J., & Kober, H. (2011). Meditation experience is associated with differences in default mode network activity and connectivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(50), 20254–20259.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Alluri, V., Toiviainen, P., Jaaskelainen, I. P., Glerean, E., Sams, M., & Brattico, E. (2012). Large-scale brain networks emerge from dynamic processing of musical timbre, key and rhythm. NeuroImage, 59(4), 3677–3689.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Minati, L., Grisoli, M., Franceschetti, S., et al. (2012). Neural signatures of economic parameters during decision-making: A functional MRI (FMRI), electroencephalography (EEG) and autonomic monitoring study. Brain Topography, 25(1), 73–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Santhanam, P., Coles, C. D., Li, Z., Li, L., Lynch, M. E., & Hu, X. (2011). Default mode network dysfunction in adults with prenatal alcohol exposure. Psychiatry Research, 194(3), 354–362.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Zuo, X. N., & Xing, X. X. (2011). Effects of non-local diffusion on structural MRI preprocessing and default ­network mapping: Statistical comparisons with isotropic/anisotropic diffusion. PLoS One, 6(10), e26703.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Buckner, R. L. (2012). The serendipitous discovery of the brain’s default network. NeuroImage, 62(2), 1137–1145.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Schinkel, S., Zamora-Lopez, G., Dimigen, O., Sommer, W., & Kurths, J. (2011). Functional network analysis reveals differences in the semantic priming task. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 197(2), 333–339.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Lavallee, C. F., Hunter, M. D., & Persinger, M. A. (2011). Intracerebral source generators characterizing concentrative meditation. Cognitive Processing, 12(2), 141–150.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Marquand, A. F., De Simoni, S., O’Daly, O. G., Williams, S. C., Mourao-Miranda, J., & Mehta, M. A. (2011). Pattern classification of working memory networks reveals differential effects of methylphenidate, atomoxetine, and placebo in healthy volunteers. Neuropsychopharmacology, 36(6), 1237–1247.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Wu, X., Li, R., Fleisher, A. S., et al. (2011). Altered default mode network connectivity in Alzheimer’s disease—A resting functional MRI and Bayesian network study. Human Brain Mapping, 32(11), 1868–1881.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Grimm, S., Ernst, J., Boesiger, P., Schuepbach, D., Boeker, H., & Northoff, G. (2011). Reduced negative BOLD responses in the default-mode network and increased self-focus in depression. The World Journal of Biological Psychiatry, 12(8), 627–637.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Beason-Held, L. L. (2011). Dementia and the default mode. Current Alzheimer Research, 8(4), 361–365.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Uddin, L. Q. (2011). The self in autism: An emerging view from neuroimaging. Neurocase, 17(3), 201–208.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Deshpande, G., Santhanam, P., & Hu, X. (2011). Instantaneous and causal connectivity in resting state brain networks derived from functional MRI data. NeuroImage, 54(2), 1043–1052.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Seubert, J., Kellermann, T., Loughead, J., et al. (2010). Processing of disgusted faces is facilitated by odor primes: A functional MRI study. NeuroImage, 53(2), 746–756.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Schwartz, G. E. (1973). Biofeedback as therapy. Some theoretical and practical issues. American Psychologist, 28(8), 666–673.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Shapiro, D., & Schwartz, G. E. (1972). Biofeedback and visceral learning: Clinical applications. Seminars in Psychiatry, 4(2), 171–184.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Hafner, R. J. (1982). Psychological treatment of essential hypertension: A controlled comparison of meditation and meditation plus biofeedback. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 7(3), 305–316.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Williamson, D. A., Jarrell, M. P., Monguillot, J. E., & Hutchinson, P. (1983). Comparisons of high, medium, and low feedback sensitivity for the control of heart-rate acceleration. Biofeedback and Self-Regulation, 8(1), 39–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Williamson, D. A., Monguillot, J. E., Hutchinson, P., Jarrell, M. P., & Blouin, D. (1981). Effect of feedback sensitivity upon learned heart rate acceleration. Psychophysiology, 18(6), 712–715.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Williamson, D. A., & Blanchard, E. B. (1979). Effect of feedback delay upon learned heart rate control. Psychophysiology, 16(2), 108–115.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Katkin, E. S. (1985). Blood, sweat, and tears: Individual differences in autonomic self-perception. Psychophysiology, 22(2), 125–137.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Shallice, T., & Burgess, P. (1996). The domain of supervisory processes and temporal organization of behaviour. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 351(1346), 1405–1411; discussion 1411–1402.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Jaiswal, N., Ray, W., & Slobounov, S. (2010). Encoding of visual-spatial information in working memory requires more cerebral efforts than retrieval: Evidence from an EEG and virtual reality study. Brain Research, 1347, 80–89.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error. New York, NY: Harper Collins.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Damasio, A. R. (2010). Self comes to mind: Constructing the conscious brain. New York, NY: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  40. Pribram, K. H. (1978). Consciousness and neurophysiology. Federation Proceedings, 37(9), 2271–2274.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Shallice, T. (1972). Dual functions of consciousness. Psychological Review, 79(5), 383–393.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1971). The control of short-term memory. Scientific American, 224, 82–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Turvey, M. (1977). Preliminaries to a theory of action with reference to vision. In R. Shaw & J. Bransford (Eds.), Perceiving, acting and knowing: Toward an ecological psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  45. Bluhm, R. L., Osuch, E. A., Lanius, R. A., et al. (2008). Default mode network connectivity: Effects of age, sex, and analytic approach. Neuroreport, 19(8), 887–891.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Buckner, R. L., & Vincent, J. L. (2007). Unrest at rest: Default activity and spontaneous network correlations. NeuroImage, 37(4), 1091–1096; discussion 1097–1099.

    Google Scholar 

  47. Buckner, R. L., & Carroll, D. C. (2007). Self-projection and the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 49–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Esposito, F., Bertolino, A., Scarabino, T., et al. (2006). Independent component model of the default-mode brain function: Assessing the impact of active thinking. Brain Research Bulletin, 70(4–6), 263–269.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Golland, Y., Bentin, S., Gelbard, H., et al. (2007). Extrinsic and intrinsic systems in the posterior cortex of the human brain revealed during natural sensory stimulation. Cerebral Cortex, 17(4), 766–777.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Greicius, M. D., & Menon, V. (2004). Default-mode activity during a passive sensory task: Uncoupled from deactivation but impacting activation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(9), 1484–1492.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Greicius, M. D., Krasnow, B., Reiss, A. L., & Menon, V. (2003). Functional connectivity in the resting brain: A network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100(1), 253–258.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Kompus, K. (2011). Default mode network gates the retrieval of task-irrelevant incidental memories. Neuroscience Letters, 487(3), 318–321.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Noirhomme, Q., Tshibanda, L. J., et al. (2010). Default network connectivity reflects the level of consciousness in non-communicative brain-damaged patients. Brain, 133(Pt 1), 161–171.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Travis, F., Haaga, D. A., Hagelin, J., et al. (2010). A self-referential default brain state: Patterns of coherence, power, and eLORETA sources during eyes-closed rest and Transcendental Meditation practice. Cognitive Processing, 11(1), 21–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Horovitz, S. G., Braun, A. R., Carr, W. S., et al. (2009). Decoupling of the brain’s default mode network during deep sleep. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(27), 11376–11381.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Cauda, F., Micon, B. M., Sacco, K., et al. (2009). Disrupted intrinsic functional connectivity in the vegetative state. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 80(4), 429–431.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Horovitz, S. G., Fukunaga, M., de Zwart, J. A., et al. (2008). Low frequency BOLD fluctuations during resting wakefulness and light sleep: A simultaneous EEG-fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 29(6), 671–682.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Liotti, M., Brannan, S., Egan, G., et al. (2001). Brain responses associated with consciousness of breathlessness (air hunger). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98(4), 2035–2040.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Brannan, S., Liotti, M., Egan, G., et al. (2001). Neuroimaging of cerebral activations and deactivations associated with hypercapnia and hunger for air. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98(4), 2029–2034.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Yoshii, N., & Fukuda, S. (1976). Several clinical aspects of thalamic pulvinotomy. Applied Neurophysiology, 39(3–4), 162–164.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Zhang, S., & Li, C. S. (2012). Functional connectivity mapping of the human precuneus by resting state fMRI. NeuroImage, 59(4), 3548–3562.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Zhang, Z., Liao, W., Chen, H., et al. (2011). Altered functional-structural coupling of large-scale brain networks in idiopathic generalized epilepsy. Brain, 134(Pt 10), 2912–2928.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Song, M., Du, H., Wu, N., et al. (2011). Impaired resting-state functional integrations within default mode network of generalized tonic-clonic seizures epilepsy. PLoS One, 6(2), e17294.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Smyser, C. D., Inder, T. E., Shimony, J. S., et al. (2010). Longitudinal analysis of neural network development in preterm infants. Cerebral Cortex, 20(12), 2852–2862.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Zhang, Z., Lu, G., Zhong, Y., et al. (2010). fMRI study of mesial temporal lobe epilepsy using amplitude of low-frequency fluctuation analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 31(12), 1851–1861.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Martuzzi, R., Ramani, R., Qiu, M., Rajeevan, N., & Constable, R. T. (2010). Functional connectivity and alterations in baseline brain state in humans. NeuroImage, 49(1), 823–834.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Habas, C. (2010). Functional connectivity of the human rostral and caudal cingulate motor areas in the brain resting state at 3T. Neuroradiology, 52(1), 47–59.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Boly, M., Tshibanda, L., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., et al. (2009). Functional connectivity in the default network during resting state is preserved in a vegetative but not in a brain dead patient. Human Brain Mapping, 30(8), 2393–2400.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Kim, D. I., Mathalon, D. H., Ford, J. M., et al. (2009). Auditory oddball deficits in schizophrenia: An independent component analysis of the fMRI multisite function BIRN study. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 35(1), 67–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Williamson, P. (2007). Are anticorrelated networks in the brain relevant to schizophrenia? Schizophrenia Bulletin, 33(4), 994–1003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Greicius, M. D., Flores, B. H., Menon, V., et al. (2007). Resting-state functional connectivity in major depression: Abnormally increased contributions from subgenual cingulate cortex and thalamus. Biological Psychiatry, 62(5), 429–437.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Lashley, K. S. (1929). Brain mechanisms and intelligence: A quantitative study of injuries to the brain. Chicago: Chicago University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  73. Sager, O. (1972). The role of reticular formation in integration. Revue Roumaine de Neurologie, 9(6), 373–380.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Merker, B. (2007). Consciousness without a cerebral cortex: a challenge for neuroscience and medicine. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(1), 63–81; discussion 81–134.

    Google Scholar 

  75. The Multi-Society Task Force on PVS. (1994). Medical aspects of the persistent vegetative state (2). The New England Journal of Medicine, 330(22), 1572–1579.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Barrett, A. M., Eslinger, P. J., Ballentine, N. H., & Heilman, K. M. (2005). Unawareness of cognitive deficit (cognitive anosognosia) in probable AD and control subjects. Neurology, 64(4), 693–699.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Plum, F. (1972). Organic disturbances of consciousness. In M. C. J. L. O’Leary (Ed.), Scientific foundations of neurology. Philadelphia: F.A. Davis.

    Google Scholar 

  78. Adair, J. C., Schwartz, R. L., Na, D. L., Fennell, E., Gilmore, R. L., & Heilman, K. M. (1997). Anosognosia: Examining the disconnection hypothesis. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 63(6), 798–800.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Lu, L. H., Barrett, A. M., Schwartz, R. L., et al. (1997). Anosognosia and confabulation during the Wada test. Neurology, 49(5), 1316–1322.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Adair, J. C., Na, D. L., Schwartz, R. L., Fennell, E. M., Gilmore, R. L., & Heilman, K. M. (1995). Anosognosia for hemiplegia: Test of the personal neglect hypothesis. Neurology, 45(12), 2195–2199.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Shuren, J. E., Hammond, C. S., Maher, L. M., Rothi, L. J., & Heilman, K. M. (1995). Attention and anosognosia: The case of a jargonaphasic patient with unawareness of language deficit. Neurology, 45(2), 376–378.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  82. Adair, J. C., Gilmore, R. L., Fennell, E. B., Gold, M., & Heilman, K. M. (1995). Anosognosia during intracarotid barbiturate anesthesia: Unawareness or amnesia for weakness. Neurology, 45(2), 241–243.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Gold, M., Adair, J. C., Jacobs, D. H., & Heilman, K. M. (1994). Anosognosia for hemiplegia: An electrophysiologic investigation of the feed-forward hypothesis. Neurology, 44(10), 1804–1808.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Gilmore, R. L., Heilman, K. M., Schmidt, R. P., Fennell, E. M., & Quisling, R. (1992). Anosognosia during Wada testing. Neurology, 42(4), 925–927.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Heilman, K. M., Barrett, A. M., & Adair, J. C. (1998). Possible mechanisms of anosognosia: A defect in self-awareness. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 353(1377), 1903–1909.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Hier, D. B., Mondlock, J., & Caplan, L. R. (1983). Behavioral abnormalities after right hemisphere stroke. Neurology, 33(3), 337–344.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  87. Cohen, R. A., Kaplan, R. F., Meadows, M. E., & Wilkinson, H. (1994). Habituation and sensitization of the orienting response following bilateral anterior cingulotomy. Neuropsychologia, 32(5), 609–617.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Cohen, R. A., Kaplan, R. F., Moser, D. J., Jenkins, M. A., & Wilkinson, H. (1999). Impairments of attention after cingulotomy. Neurology, 53(4), 819–824.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Cohen, R. A., Paul, R., Zawacki, T. M., Moser, D. J., Sweet, L., & Wilkinson, H. (2001). Emotional and personality changes following cingulotomy. Emotion (Washington, DC), 1(1), 38–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  90. Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology. New York: Appleton.

    Google Scholar 

  91. Neisser, U. (1976). Cognition and reality. San Francisco: W.H. Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  92. Hochberg, J. E. (1970). Attention, organization, and consciousness. In D. I. Mostofsky (Ed.), Attention, contemporary theory and analysis (pp. 99–124). New York: Appleton.

    Google Scholar 

  93. Hochberg, D., Molina-Paris, C., Perez-Mercader, J., & Visser, M. (1999). Effective action for stochastic partial differential equations. Physical Review, 60(6 Pt A), 6343–6360.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  94. Hochberg, J. (2003). Acts of perceptual inquiry: Problems for any stimulus-based simplicity theory. Acta Psychologica, 114(3), 215–228.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  95. Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1979). Automatic and effortful processes in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 108, 356–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  96. Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1984). Automatic processing of fundamental information: The case of frequency of occurrence. American Psychologist, 39, 1372–1388.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  97. Norman, D., & Shallice, T. (1984). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behavior. In R. Davidson, G. E. Schwartz, & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation (Vol. 4, pp. 3–16). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2014 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Cohen, R.A. (2014). Consciousness and Self-Directed Attention. In: The Neuropsychology of Attention. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-72639-7_21

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-72639-7_21

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Boston, MA

  • Print ISBN: 978-0-387-72638-0

  • Online ISBN: 978-0-387-72639-7

  • eBook Packages: MedicineMedicine (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics