Effects of Harmonicity and Regularity on the Perception of Sound Sources

  • Robert P. Carlyon
  • Hedwig E. Gockel
Part of the Springer Handbook of Auditory Research book series (SHAR, volume 29)


Complex Tone Stream Segregation Pitch Perception Auditory Filter Harmonic Complex 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.


Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.


  1. Alain C, Arnott SR Picton TW (2001) Bottom-up and top-down influences on auditory scene analysis: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. J Exp Psychol [Hum Percep Perform] 27:1072–1089.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Anstis S, Saida S (1985) Adaptation to auditory streaming of frequency-modulated tones. J Exp Psychol [Hum Percep Perform] 11:257–271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Assmann P, Summerfield Q (1990) Modeling the perception of concurrent vowels: Vowels with different fundamental frequencies. J Acoust Soc Am 88:680–697.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Beauvois MW, Meddis R (1996) Computer simulation of auditory stream segregation in alternating-tone sequences. J Acoust Soc Am 99:2270–2280.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Beerends JG, Houtsma AJM (1989) Pitch identification of simultaneous diotic and dichotic two-tone complexes. J Acoust Soc Am 85:813–819.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bernstein JGW, Oxenham AJ (2005) An autocorrelation model with place dependence to account for the effect of harmonic number on fundamental frequency discrimination. J Acoust Soc Am 117:3816–3831.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bregman AS (1978) Auditory streaming is cumulative. J Exp Psychol [Hum Percep Perform] 4:380–387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bregman AS (1990) Auditory Scene Analysis: The Perceptual Organization of Sound. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books, MIT Press.Google Scholar
  9. Bregman AS, Campbell J (1971) Primary auditory stream segregation and perception of order in rapid sequences of tones. J Exp Psychol 89:244–249.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Bregman AS, Liao C Levitan R (1990) Auditory grouping based on fundamental frequency and formant peak frequency. Can J Psychol 44:400–413.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Bregman AS, Ahad PA, Crum PAC, O’Reilly J (2000) Effects of time intervals and tone durations on auditory stream segregation. Percept Psychophys 62:626–636.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Brokx JPL, Nooteboom SG (1982) Intonation and the perceptual separation of simultaneous voices. J Phonet 10:23–36.Google Scholar
  13. Brunstrom JM, Roberts B (2000) Separate mechanisms govern the selection of spectral components for perceptual fusion and for the computation of global pitch. J Acoust Soc Am 107:1566–1577.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Carlyon RP (1994) Detecting mistuning in the presence of synchronous and asynchronous interfering sounds. J Acoust Soc Am 95:2622–2630.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Carlyon RP (1996) Encoding the fundamental frequency of a complex tone in the presence of a spectrally overlapping masker. J Acoust Soc Am 99:517–524.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Carlyon RP (2004) How the brain separates sounds. Trends Cogn Sci 8:435–478.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Carlyon RP, Deeks JM (2002) Limitations on rate discrimination. J Acoust Soc Am 112:1009–1025.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Carlyon RP, Shackleton TM (1994) Comparing the fundamental frequencies of resolved and unresolved harmonics: Evidence for two pitch mechanisms? J Acoust Soc Am 95:3541–3554.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Carlyon RP, Cusack R, Foxton JM, Robertson IH (2001) Effects of attention and unilateral neglect on auditory stream segregation. Exp Psychol [Human Percept Perform] 27: 115–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Culling JF, Darwin CJ (1994) Perceptual and computational separation of simultaneous vowels: cues arising from low-frequency beating. J Acoust Soc Am 95:1559–1569.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Cusack R, Carlyon RP (2000) Auditory pop-out: Perceptual asymmetries in sequences of sounds. Br J Audiol 34:112.Google Scholar
  22. Cusack R, Deeks J, Aikman G, Carlyon RP (2004) Effects of location, frequency region, and time course of selective attention on auditory scene analysis. J Exp Psychol [Hum Percept Perform] 30:643–656.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Dannenbring GL, Bregman AS (1978) Streaming vs. fusion of sinusoidal components of complex tones. Percept Psychophys 24:369–376.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Darwin CJ (1992) Listening to two things at once. In: Schouten B (ed) Audition, Speech, and Language. Berlin: Mouton-De Gruyter, pp. 133–148.Google Scholar
  25. Darwin CJ (1997) Auditory grouping. Trends Cogn Sci 1:327–333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Darwin CJ, Carlyon RP (1995) Auditory grouping. In: Moore BCJ (ed) Hearing. San Diego: Academic Press, pp. 387–424.Google Scholar
  27. Darwin CJ, Hukin RW (1999) Auditory objects of attention: The role of interaural time-differences. J Exp Psychol [Hum Percept Perform] 25:617–629.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Darwin CJ, Pattison H Gardner RB (1989) Vowel quality changes produced by surrounding tone sequences. Percept Psychophys 45:333–342.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. de Cheveigné A (1993) Separation of concurrent harmonic sounds: Fundamental frequency estimation and a time-domain cancellation model of auditory processing. J Acoust Soc Am 93:3271–3290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Deeks JM, Carlyon RP (2004) Simulations of cochlear implant hearing using filtered harmonic complexes: Implications for concurrent sound segregation. J Acoust Soc Am 115:1736–1746.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Dowling WJ (1973) The perception of interleaved melodies. Cogn Psychol 5:322–337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Feth L (1974) Frequency discrimination of complex periodic tones. Percept Psychophys 15:375–379.Google Scholar
  33. Fishman YI, Reser DH, Arezzo JC, Steinschneider M (2001) Neural correlates of auditory stream segregation in primary auditory cortex of the awake monkey. Hear Res 151: 167–187.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Glasberg BR, Moore BCJ (1990) Derivation of auditory filter shapes from notched-noise data. Hear Res 47:103–138.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Gockel H, Carlyon RP (1998) Effects of ear of entry and perceived location of synchronous and asynchronous components on mistuning detection. J Acoust Soc Am 104:534–3545.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Gockel H, Carlyon RP Micheyl C (1999) Context dependence of fundamental-frequency discrimination: Lateralized temporal fringes. J Acoust Soc Am 106:3553–3563.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Gockel H, Carlyon RP Plack CJ (2004) Across-frequency interference effects in fundamental frequency discrimination: Questioning evidence for two pitch mechanisms. J Acoust Soc Am 116:1092–1104.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Gockel H, Carlyon RP Moore BCJ (2005) Pitch discrimination interference: The role of pitch pulse asynchrony. J Acoust Soc Am 117:3860–3866.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Godsmark D, Brown GJ (1999) A blackboard architecture for computational auditory scene analysis. Speech Commun 27:351–366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Goldstein J L (1973) An optimum processor theory for the central formation of the pitch of complex tones. J Acoust Soc Am 54:1496–1516.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Grimault N, Micheyl C, Carlyon RP, Arthaud P Collet L (2000) Influence of peripheral resolvability on the perceptual segregation of harmonic complex tones differing in fundamental frequency. J Acoust Soc Am 108:263–271.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Hartmann WM, Johnson D (1991) Stream segregation and peripheral channeling. Music Percept 9:155–184.Google Scholar
  43. Hartmann WM, McAdams S, Smith BK (1990) Hearing a mistuned harmonic in an otherwise periodic complex tone. J Acoust Soc Am 88:1712–1724.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Hill NJ, Darwin CJ (1996) Lateralization of a perturbed harmonic: Effects of onset asynchrony and mistuning. J Acoust Soc Am 100:2352–2364.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Houtsma AJM, Smurzynski J (1990) J.F. Schouten revisited: Pitch of complex tones having many high-order harmonics. J Acoust Soc Am 87:304–310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Kim DO, Molnar CE Matthews JW (1980) Cochlear mechanics: Nonlinear behavior in two-tone responses as reflected in cochlear-nerve-fiber responses and in ear-canal sound pressure. J Acoust Soc Am 67:1704–1721.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Licklider JCR (1951) A duplex theory of pitch perception. Experientia 7:128–133.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Licklider JCR (1956) Auditory frequency analysis. In: Cherry C (ed) Information Theory. New York: Academic Press, pp. 253–268.Google Scholar
  49. Lyzenga J, Carlyon RP (1999) Center frequency modulation detection for harmonic complexes resembling vowel formants and its interference by off-frequency maskers. J Acoust Soc Am 105:2792–2806.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. McCabe SL, Denham MJ (1997) A model of auditory streaming. J Acoust Soc Am 101:1611–1621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Meddis R, Hewitt M (1991) Virtual pitch and phase sensitivity studied using a computer model of the auditory periphery I: Pitch identification. J Acoust Soc Am 89:2866–2882.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Meddis R, O’Mard L (1997) A unitary model of pitch perception. J Acoust Soc Am 102:1811–1820.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Micheyl C, Carlyon RP (1998) Effects of temporal fringes on fundamental-frequency discrimination. J Acoust Soc Am 104:3006–3018.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Micheyl C, Tian B, Carlyon RP, Rauschecker JP (2005) Perceptual organization of tone sequences in the auditory cortex of awake macaques. Neuron 48:139–148.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Miller GA, Heise GA (1950) The trill threshold. J Acoust Soc Am 22:637–638.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Moore BCJ (1982) An Introduction to the Psychology of Hearing, 2nd ed. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  57. Moore BCJ (2003) An Introduction to the Psychology of Hearing, 5th ed. San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  58. Moore BCJ, Carlyon RP (2005) Perception of pitch by people with cochlear hearing loss and by cochlear implant users. In: Plack CJ, Oxenham AJ (eds) Springer Handbook of Auditory Research: Pitch Perception. New York: Springer-Verlag, pp. 234–277.Google Scholar
  59. Moore BCJ, Gockel H (2002) Factors influencing sequential stream segregation. Acust Acta Acust 88:320–333.Google Scholar
  60. Moore BCJ, Ohgushi K (1993) Audibility of partials in inharmonic complex tones. J Acoust Soc Am 93:452–461.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Moore BCJ, Glasberg BR Peters RW (1985) Relative dominance of individual partials in determining the pitch of complex tones. J Acoust Soc Am 77:1853–1860.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Moore BCJ, Glasberg BR, Peters RW (1986) Thresholds for hearing mistuned partials as separate tones in harmonic complexes. J Acoust Soc Am 80:479–483.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Oxenham AJ, Bernstein JGW Penagos H (2004) Correct tonotopic representation is necessary for complex pitch perception. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101:1421–1425.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Patterson R D, Allerhand M, Giguére C (1995) Time-domain modelling of peripheral auditory processing: A modular architecture and a software platform. J Acoust Soc Am 98:1890–1894.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Plack CJ, Oxenham AJ, Fay RR, Popper AN (eds) (2005) Pitch: Neural Coding and Perception. New York: springer.Google Scholar
  66. Plomp R (1964) The ear as a frequency analyzer. J Acoust Soc Am 36:1628–1636.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Plomp R (1967) Pitch of complex tones. J Acoust Soc Am 41:1526–1533.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Plomp R (1976) Aspects of Tone Sensation. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  69. Roberts B (2005) Spectral pattern, grouping, and the pitches of complex tones and their components. Acust Acta Acust 91:945–957.Google Scholar
  70. Roberts B, Bregman AS (1991) Effects of the pattern of spectral spacing on the perceptual fusion of harmonics. J Acoust Soc Am 90:3050–3060.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Roberts B, Brunstrom JM (1998) Perceptual segregation and pitch shifts of mistuned components in harmonic complexes and in regular inharmonic complexes. J Acoust Soc Am 104:2326–2338.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Roberts B, Brunstrom JM (2001) Perceptual fusion and fragmentation of complex tones made inharmonic by applying different degrees of frequency shift and spectral stretch. J Acoust Soc Am 110:2479–2490.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Roberts B, Brunstrom JM (2003) Spectral pattern, harmonic relations, and the perceptual grouping of low-numbered components. J Acoust Soc Am 114:2118–2134.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Rose MM, Moore BCJ (1997) Perceptual grouping of tone sequences by normally hearing and hearing-impaired listeners. J Acoust Soc Am 102:1768–1778.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Scheffers MTM (1983) Sifting vowels: auditory pitch analysis and sound segregation. PhD thesis, University of Groningen, Netherlands.Google Scholar
  76. Schouten JF, Ritsma RJ Cardozo BL (1962) Pitch of the residue. J Acoust Soc Am 34:1418–1424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Shamma S (1985) Speech Processing in the Auditory System: II. Lateral inhibition and the central processing of speech evoked activity in the auditory nerve. J Acoust Soc Am 78:1622–1632.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Shannon RV (1983) Multichannel electrical stimulation of the auditory nerve in man. I. Basic psychophysics. Hear Res 11:157–189.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Singh PG (1987) Perceptual organization of complex-tone sequences: A tradeoff between pitch and timbre? J Acoust Soc Am 82:886–895.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Sussman E, Winkler I, Huotilainen M, Ritter W Naatanen R (2002) Top-down effects can modify the initially stimulus-driven auditory organization. Cogn Brain Res 13:393–405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. van Noorden LPAS (1975) Temporal coherence in the perception of tone sequences. PhD thesis, Eindhoven University of Technology.Google Scholar
  82. Vliegen J, Moore BCJ Oxenham AJ (1999) The role of spectral and periodicity cues in auditory stream segregation, measured using a temporal discrimination task. J Acoust Soc Am 106:938–945.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Vliegen J, Oxenham AJ (1999) Sequential stream segregation in the absence of spectral cues. J Acoust Soc Am 105:339–346.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  • Robert P. Carlyon
  • Hedwig E. Gockel

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations