Auditory Attention and Filters

  • Ervin R. Hafter
  • Anastasios Sarampalis
  • Psyche Loui
Part of the Springer Handbook of Auditory Research book series (SHAR, volume 29)


Blood Oxygenation Level Dependent Ideal Observer Auditory Attention Stream Segregation Auditory Stream 


Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.


  1. Bhawrucha JJ, Stoeckig K (1986) Reaction-time and musical expectancy—priming of chords. J Exp Psychol [Hum Percept Perform] 12:403–410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bonnel AM, and Hafter ER (1998) Divided attention between simultaneous auditory and visual signals. Perception and Psychophysics 60(2), 179–190.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Bregman AS (1990) Auditory Scene Analysis: The Perceptual Organization of Sound. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books, MIT Press.Google Scholar
  4. Broadbent DE (1958) Perception and Communication. New York: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
  5. Carlyon RP, Cusack R, Foxton JM, Robertson IH (2001) Effects of attention and unilateral neglect on auditory stream segregation. J Exp Psychol [Hum Percept Perform] 27: 115–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Carlyon RP, Plack CJ, Fantini DA, Cusack R (2003) Cross-modal and non-sensory influences on auditory streaming. Perception 32:1393–1402.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Cherry EC (1953) Some experiments on the recognition of speech with one and with two ears. J Acoust Soc Am 24:975–979.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Dai H, Wright BA (1995) Detecting signals of unexpected or uncertain durations. J Acoust Soc Am 98:798–806.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Dai HP, Scharf B, Buus S (1991) Effective attenuation of signals in noise under focused attention. J Acoust Soc Am 89:2837–2842.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Deutsch JA, Deutsch D (1963) Attention: Some theoretical considerations. Psychol Rev 70:80–90.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Dowling WJ (1973) The perception of interleaved melodies. Cogn Psych 5:322–337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Dowling WJ, Lung KM-T, Herrbold S (1987) Aiming attention in pitch and time in the perception of interleaved melodies. Percept Psychophys 41:642–656.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  13. Egan JP, Greenberg GZ, Schulman AI (1961) Intervals of time uncertainty in auditory detection. J Acoust Soc Am 33:771–778.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Fletcher H (1940) Auditory patterns. Rev Mod Phys 12:47–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Green DM (1960) Psychoacoustics and detection theory. J Acoust Soc Am 32:1189–1203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Green DM (1961) Detection of auditory sinusoids of uncertainty frequency. J Acoust Soc Am 33:897–903.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Green DM, Swets JA (1966) Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics. New York: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
  18. Green TJ, McKeown JD (2001) Capture of attention in selective frequency listening. J Exp Psychol [Hum Percept Perform] 27:1197–1210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Greenberg GZ, Larkin WD (1968) Frequency-response characteristic of auditory observers detecting signals of a single frequency in noise: The probe-signal method. J Acoust Soc Am 44:1513–1523.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Greenwood DD (1961) Critical bandwidth and the frequency coordinates of the basilar membrane. J Acoust Soc Am 33:1344–1356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Gundy RF (1961) Auditory detection of an unspecified signal. J Acoust Soc Am 33: 1008–1012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hackley SA (1993) An evaluation of the automaticity of sensory processing using event-related potentials and brainstem reflexes. Psychophysiology 30:415–428.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Hafter ER, Kaplan R (1976) The interaction between motivation and uncertainty as a factor in detection. NASA project report, Ames Research Center, Moffit Field, CA.Google Scholar
  24. Hafter ER, Saberi K (2001) A level of stimulus representation model for auditory detection and attention. J Acoust Soc Am 110:1489–1497.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Hafter ER, Schlauch RS (1989) Factors in detection under uncertainty. J Acoust Soc Am 86:S112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Hafter ER, Schlauch RS, Tang J (1993) Attending to auditory filters that were not stimulated directly. J Acoust Soc Am 94:743–747.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hartmann WM (1997) Signals, Sounds, and Sensation. New York: AIP Press.Google Scholar
  28. Hartmann WM, Johnson D (1991) Stream segregation and peripheral channeling. Music Percept 9:155–184.Google Scholar
  29. Hillyard SA, Hink RF, Schwent VL, Picton TW (1973) Electrical signs of selective attention in the human brain. Science 182:177–80.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Houtsma AJM (2004) Hawkins and Stevens revisited with insert earphones (L). J Acoust Soc Am 115:967–970.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Ison JR, Virag TM, Allen PD, Hammond GR (2002) The attention filter for tones in noise has the same shape and effective bandwidth in the elderly as it has in young listeners. J Acoust Soc Am 112:238–246.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Janata P, Tillmann B, Bharucha JJ (2002) Listening to polyphonic music recruits domain-general attention and working memory circuits. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 2: 121–140.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Jeffress LA (1964) Stimulus-oriented approach to detection. J Acoust Soc Am 36: 766–774.Google Scholar
  34. Jeffress LA (1970) Masking. In: Tobias J (ed) Foundations of Modern Auditory Theory 1. New York: Academic Press, pp. 87–114.Google Scholar
  35. Jeffress LA, Blodgett HC, Sandel TT, Wood CL III (1956) Masking of tonal signals. J Acoust Soc Am 3:416–426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Johnson DM, Hafter ER (1980) Uncertain-frequency detection—–Cueing and condition of observation. Percept Psychophys 28:143–149.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Jones MR, Boltz M (1989) Dynamic attending and responses to time. Psychol Rev 96: 459–491.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Jones MR, Moynihan H, MacKenzie N, Puente J (2002) Temporal aspects of stimulus-driven attending in dynamic arrays. Psychol Sci 13:313–319.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Jonides J (1981) Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind’s eye. In: Long J, Baddeley AD (eds) Attention and Performance IX. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  40. Kahneman D (1973) Attention and Effort. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  41. Krumhansl CL (1990) Tonal hierarchies and rare intervals in music cognition. Music Percept 7:309–324.Google Scholar
  42. Krumhansl CL, Kessler EJ (1982) Tracing the dynamic changes in perceived tonal organization in a spatial representation of musical keys. Psychol Rev 89:334–368.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Large EW, Jones MR (1999) The dynamics of attending: How people track time-varying events. Psychol Rev 106:119–159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Leshowitz B, Wightman FL (1972) On the importance of considering the signal’s frequency spectrum: Some comments on Macmillan’s Detection and recognition of increments and decrements in auditory intensity experiment. Percept Psychophys 12:209–212.Google Scholar
  45. Macmillan NA (1971) Detection and recognition of increments and decrements in auditory intensity. Perception and Psychophysics, 10:233–238.Google Scholar
  46. Meyer L (1956) Emotion and Meaning in Music. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  47. Mondor TA and Lacey TE (2001) Facilitative and inhibitory effects of cuing, sound duration, intensity, and timbre. Percept Psychophys 63:726–736.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Moore BCJ (2003) An Introduction to the Psychology of Hearing, 5th ed. San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  49. Moore BCJ, Glasberg BR (1983) Suggested formulas for calculating auditory-filter bandwidths and excitation patterns. J Acoust Soc Am 74:750–753.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Moore BCJ, Gockel H (2002) Factors influencing sequential stream segregation. Acta Acust 88:320–333.Google Scholar
  51. Moray N (1960) Broadbent’s filter theory—postulate H and the problem of switching time. Q J Exp Psychol 12:214–220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Moray N (1970) Attention: Selective Processes in Vision and Hearing. New York: Academic Press,.Google Scholar
  53. Näätänen R. 1988. Implications of ERP data for psychological theories of attention. Biological Psychology, 26(1–3):117–63PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Norman DA (1969) Memory while shadowing. Q J Exp Psychol 21:85–93.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Norman DA, Bobrow DG (1975) On data-limited and resource-limited processes. Cogn Psych 7:44–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Patterson RD, Moore BCJ (1986) Auditory filters and excitation patterns as representations of frequency resolution. In: Moore BCJ (ed) Frequency Selectivity in Hearing. New York: Academic Press, pp. 123–177.Google Scholar
  57. Patterson RD, Nimmo-Smith I (1980) Off-frequency listening and auditory-filter asymmetry. J Acoust Soc Am 67:229–245.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Penner MJ (1972) Effects of payoffs and cue tones on detection of sinusoids of uncertain frequency. Percept Psychophys 11:198–202.Google Scholar
  59. Petkov CI, Kang X, Alho K, Bertrand O, Yund EW, and Woods DL (2004) Attentional modulation of human auditory cortex. Nat Neurosci 7:658–663.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Plamondon L, Hafter ER (1990) Selective attention in absolute pitch listeners. J Acoust Soc Am Suppl 1 88:S49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Posner MI (1980) Orienting of attention. Q J Exp Psychol 32:3–25.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Posner MI, Cohen Y (1984) Components of visual orienting. In: Bouma H, Bouwhuis DG (eds) Attention & Performance X. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 531–555.Google Scholar
  63. Scharf B (1970) Critical Bands. In: Tobias J (ed) Foundations of Modern Auditory Theory 1. New York: Academic Press, pp. 159–202.Google Scholar
  64. Scharf B, Quigly S, Aoki C, Peachy N, Reeves A (1987) Focused auditory attention and frequency selectivity. Percept Psychophys 42:215–223.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  65. Scharf B, Magnan J, Chays A (1997) On the role of the olivocochlear bundle in hearing: 16 case studies. Hear Res 103:101–122.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Schlauch RS, Hafter ER (1991) Listening bandwidths and frequency uncertainty in pure-tone signal detection. J Acoust Soc Am 90:1332–1339.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Spence CJ, Driver J (1994) Covert spatial orienting in audition: Exogenous and endogenous mechanisms. J Exp Psychol [Hum Percept Perform] 20:555–574.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Spence C, Driver J (1998) Auditory and audiovisual inhibition of return. Percept Psychophys 60:125–139.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  69. Spence C, Driver J (2000) Attracting attention to the illusory location of a sound: Reflexive crossmodal orienting and ventriloquism. NeuroReport 11:2057–2061.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. Sussman E, Ritter W, Vaughan HG Jr (1998) Attention affects the organization of auditory input associated with the mismatch negativity system. Brain Res 789:130–138.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Sussman E, Ritter W, Vaughan HG Jr (1999) An investigation of the auditory streaming effect using event-related potentials. Psychophysiology 36:22–34PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Swets JA (1984) Mathematical models of attention. In: Parasuraman R, Davies DR (eds) Varieties of Attention. London: Academic Press, pp. 183–242.Google Scholar
  73. Taylor MM, Forbes SM (1969) Monaural detection with contralateral cue (MDCC). I. Better than energy detector performance by human observers. J Acoust Soc Am 46:1519–1526.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Treisman AM (1964) Monitoring and storage of irrelevant messages in selective attention. J Verb Learn Verb Behav 3:449–459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Treisman AM (1969) Strategies and models of selective attention. Psychol Rev 76: 282–299.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Treisman AM, Gelade G (1980) Feature-integration theory of attention. Cogn Psycho 12:97–136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. van Noorden LPAS (1975) Temporal coherence in the perception of tone sequences. PhD thesis, Eindhoven University of Technology.Google Scholar
  78. Wessel DL (1979) Timbre space as a musical control structure. Comp Music J 3:45–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Woldorff MG, Hillyard SA (1991) Modulation of early auditory processing during selective listening to rapidly presented tones. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 79:170–191.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Woldorff MG, Hillyard SA, Gallen CC, Hampson SR, Bloom FE (1998) Magnetoencephalographic recordings demonstrate attentional modulation of mismatch-related neural activity in human auditory cortex. Psychophysiology 35:283– 292.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  81. Wright BA, Dai H (1994) Detection of unexpected tones with short and long durations. J Acoust Soc Am 95:931–938.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. Yost WA, Penner MJ, Feth LL (1972) Signal detection as a function of contralateral signal-to-noise ratio. J Acoust Soc Am 51:1966–1970.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. Zatorre RJ, Mondor TA, Evans AC (1999) Auditory attention to space and frequency activates similar cerebral systems. NeuroImage 10:544–554.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. Zatorre RJ, Belin P, Penhune VB (2002) Structure and function of auditory cortex: Music and speech. Trends Cogn Sci 6:37–46.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2008

Authors and Affiliations

  • Ervin R. Hafter
  • Anastasios Sarampalis
  • Psyche Loui

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations