Advertisement

Predation and Primate Cognitive Evolution

  • Klaus Zuberbühler
Chapter
Part of the Developments in Primatology: Progress and Prospects book series (DIPR)

Abstract

Predation is a major cause of mortality in non-human primates (Cheney et al., 2004), but its impact as a selective force on primate evolution is not well understood. Predation has long been thought to affect traits such as body size, group size, group composition, and ecological niche, as well as the traits of vigilance and vocal and reproductive behaviour (Anderson, 1986). The general assumption is that if a trait has evolved as an adaptation to predation, then there should be a negative relationship between the expression of the trait and the individual’s vulnerability to predation. First, if large body size is an adaptation to predation (Isbell, 1994), then larger primates should be underrepresented in a predator’s prey spectrum compared to smaller ones. Second, if individuals living in large groups are less susceptible to a certain kind of predation, leopard predation, for example, due to enhanced levels of predator vigilance, then individuals of larger groups should be underrepresented in the prey spectrum (van Schaik, 1983; Cords, 1990). Third, if multi-male groups are an adaptation to predation, for example due to the possibility of cooperative defence (Stanford, 1998), then species living in multi-male groups should be underrepresented in a predator’s prey spectrum compared to single-male groups. Fourth, if females shorten their inter-birth intervals to increase their lifetime reproductive success to compensate for higher levels of predation (Hill & Dunbar, 1998), then individuals with short inter-birth intervals should be over-represented in the prey spectrum.

Keywords

Ivory Coast Alarm Call Monkey Species Monkey Group Colobus Monkey 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Anderson, C.M. (1986). Predation and primate evolution. Primates, 27: 15–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Arnold, K., and Zuberbühler, K. (2006a). The alarm calling adult male putty-nosed monkeys, Cercopithecus nictitans martini. Animal Behaviour, 72.Google Scholar
  3. Arnold, K., and Zuberbühler, K. (2006b). Semantic combinations in primate calls. Nature, 441: 303.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bailey, T.N. (1993). The African leopard. New York: Columbia Univ. Press.Google Scholar
  5. Boesch, C. (1994). Chimpanzee-red colobus monkeys: A predator-prey system. Animal Behaviour, 47, 1135–1148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Boesch, C. (1994). Hunting strategies of Gombe and Taï chimpanzees. In R. Wrangham, W. McGrew, F. de Waal, and P. Heltne (Eds.), Chimpanzee Cultures (pp. 77–92). Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  7. Boesch, C. (2002). Cooperative hunting roles among Taï chimpanzees. Human Nature, 13: 27–46.Google Scholar
  8. Boesch, C., and Boesch, H. (1989). Hunting behavior of wild chimpanzees in the Taï National Park. Am. J. Phys. Anthr., 78: 547–573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Boesch, C., and Boesch-Achermann, H. (2000). The chimpanzees of the Taï forest. Behavioural ecology and evolution. Oxford: Oxford Univ. Press.Google Scholar
  10. Brown, L. (1982). The prey of the crowned eagle Stephanoaetus coronatus in Central Kenya. Scopus, 6: 91–94.Google Scholar
  11. Bshary, R. (2001). Diana monkeys, Cercopithecus diana, adjust their anti-predator response behaviour to human hunting strategies. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 50: 251–256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Bshary, R. (2007). Interactions between red colobus monkeys and chimpanzees. In W.S. McGraw, K. Zuberbühler & R. Noë (Eds.), Monkeys of the Taï forest: An African primate community Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press.Google Scholar
  13. Bshary, R., and Noë, R. (1997). Anti-predation behaviour of red colobus monkeys in the presence of chimpanzees. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 41: 321–333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Bshary, R., and Noë, R. (1997). Red colobus and Diana monkeys provide mutual protection against predators. Animal Behaviour, 54: 1461–1474.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Buzzard, P., & Eckardt, W. (2007). Social systems of the Taï guenons (Cercopithecus spp.). In R. McGraw, K. Zuberbühler & R. Noë (Eds.), Monkeys of the Taï forest: An African monkey community. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press.Google Scholar
  16. Cheney, D.L., Seyfarth, R.M., Fischer, J., Beehner, J., Bergman, T., Johnson, S.E., Kitchen, D.M., Palombit, R.A., Rendall, D., and Silk, J.B. (2004). Factors affecting reproduction and mortality among baboons in the Okavango Delta, Botswana. International Journal of Primatology, 25: 401–428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Cheney, D.L., and Wrangham, R.W. (1987). Predation. In B. Smuts, et al. (Eds.), Primate societies (pp. 227–239). Chicago: Univ. of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  18. Cords, M. (1990). Vigilance and mixed-species association of some East African forest monkeys. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 26: 297–300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Dind, F., Jenny, D., and Boesch, C. (1996). Écologie et prédation du léopard en foret de Taï (Côte d’Ivoire). In J.F. Graf, M. Zinsstag, and J. Zinsstag (Eds.), Centre Suisse de Recherches Scientifiques en Côte d’Ivoire (CSRS). Rapport d’activité 1992–1994 (pp. 22–26). Abidjan: Centre Suisse de Recherches Scientifiques en Côte d’Ivoire.Google Scholar
  20. Eckardt, W., and Zuberbühler, K. (2004). Cooperation and competition in two forest monkeys. Behavioral Ecology, 15: 400–411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Enstam, K.L., and Isbell, L.A. (2004). Microhabitat preference and vertical use of space by patas monkeys (Erythrocebus patas) in relation to predation risk and habitat structure. Folia Primatologica, 75: 70–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Gautier-Hion, A., and Gautier, J.P. (1974). Les associations polyspecifiques de Cercopitheques du Plateau de M’Passa (Gabon). Folia primatol., 22: 134–177.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  23. Gautier-Hion, A., and Tutin, C.E.G. (1988). Simultaneous attack by adult males of a polyspecific troop of monkeys against a crowned hawk eagle. Folia Primatol., 51:149–151.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Gil-da-Costa, R., Palleroni, A., Hauser, M.D., Touchton, J., and Kelley, J.P. (2003). Rapid acquisition of an alarm response by a neotropical primate to a newly introduced avian predator. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B–Biological Sciences, 270: 605–610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Goodall, J. (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of behavior. Cambridge: Harvard Univ. Press.Google Scholar
  26. Hart, J.A., Katembo, M., and Punga, K. (1996). Diet, prey selection and ecological relations of leopard and golden cat in the Ituri forest, Zaire. African Journal of Ecology, 34:364–379.Google Scholar
  27. Herbinger, I., Boesch, C., and Rothe, H. (2001). Territory characteristics among three neighbouring chimpanzee communities in the Taï National Park, Côte d’Ivoire. Int. J. Primatol., 22: 143–167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Hill, K., Boesch, C., Pusey, A., Williams, J., and Wrangham, R. (2001). Mortality rates among wild chimpanzees. J. Hum. Evol., 40: 437–450.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Hill, R.A., and Dunbar, R.I.M. (1998). An evaluation of the roles of predation rate and predation risk as selective pressures on primate grouping behaviour. Behaviour, 135:411–430.Google Scholar
  30. Hill, R.A., and Lee, P.C. (1998). Predation risk as an influence on group size in cercopithecoid primates: Implications for social structure. J. of Zoology, 245: 447–456.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Hoppe-Dominik, B. (1984). Etude du spectre des proies de la panthere, Panthera pardus, dans le Parc National de Taï en Côte d’Ivoire. Mammalia, 48: 477–487.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Humphrey, N.K. (1976). The social function of intellect. In P.P.G. Bateson and R.A. Hinde (Eds.), Growing points in ethology (pp. 303–317). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge Univ. Press.Google Scholar
  33. Isbell, L.A., Cheney, D.L., and Seyfarth, R.M. (1993). Are immigrant vervet monkeys, Cercopithecus aethiops, at greater risk of mortality than residents? Animal Behaviour, 45: 729–734.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Janmaat, K.R.L., Byrne, R.W., and Zuberbühler, K. (2006a). Evidence for a spatial memory of fruiting states of rainforest trees in wild mangabeys. Animal Behaviour.Google Scholar
  35. Janmaat, K.R.L., Byrne, R.W., and Zuberbühler, K. (2006b). Primates take weather into account when searching for fruits. Curr. Biol., 16: 1232–1237.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Jenny, D. (1996). Spatial organization of leopards (Panthera pardus) in Taï National Park, Ivory Coast: Is rain forest habitat a tropical haven? J. Zool., 240: 427–440.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Jenny, D., and Zuberbühler, K. (2005). Hunting behaviour in West African forest leopards. African J. of Ecology, 43: 197–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Korstjens, A.H. (2001). The mob, the secret sorority, and the phantoms: An analysis of the socio-ecological strategies of the three colobines of Taï. Unpublished Ph.D., Utrecht.Google Scholar
  39. Krebs, J.R., and Davis, N.B. (1993). Predator versus prey: Evolutionary arms races. In J.R. Krebs and N.B. Davies (Eds.), An introduction to behavioral ecology (pp. 77–101). Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications.Google Scholar
  40. Lehmann, J., and Boesch, C. (2003). Social influences on ranging patterns among chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) in the Taï National Park, Côte d’Ivoire. Behavioral Ecology, 14: 642–649CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Malan, G., and Shultz, S. (2002). Nest-site selection of the crowned hawk-eagle in the forests of KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa, and Taï, Ivory Coast. Journal of Raptor Research, 36: 300–308.Google Scholar
  42. Marchesi, P., Marchesi, N., Fruth, B., and Boesch, C. (1995). Census and distribution of chimpanzees in Côte-Divoire. Primates, 36: 591–607.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Martin, C. (1991). The rainforests of West Africa: Ecology, threats, conservation. Basel: Birkhäuser.Google Scholar
  44. McGraw, W.S. (1998). Comparative locomotion and habitat use of six monkeys in the Taï Forest, Ivory Coast. Am. J. Phys. Anthrop., 105: 493–510.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. McGraw, W.S. (2000). Positional behavior of Cercopithecus petaurista. Inter. Jour. of Primatology, 21: 157–182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Mercader, J., Panger, M., Scott-Cummings, L., and Boesch, C. (2003). Late Holocene archaeological remains from chimpanzee and human sites in the rainforest of Côte d’Ivoire. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 150–151.Google Scholar
  47. Noë, R., and Bshary, R. (1997). The formation of red colobus-Diana monkey associations under predation pressure from chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B—Biological Sciences, 264: 253–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Refisch, J. (2001). Einfluss der Wilderei auf Affen und die sekundären Effekte auf die Vegetation im Taä Nationalpark, Elfenbeinküste. In: Bayreuther Forum Ökologie (pp. 1–189). Bayreuth: Bayreuth.Google Scholar
  49. Refisch, J., and Kone, I. (2005). Impact of commercial hunting on monkey populations in the Taï region, Côte d’Ivoire. Biotropica, 37: 136–144CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Reynolds, V. (2005). The chimpanzees of the Budongo forest—Ecology, behaviour, and conservation. Oxford: Oxford Univ. Press.Google Scholar
  51. Shultz, S. (2001). Notes on interactions between monkeys and African crowned eagles in Taï National Park, Ivory Coast. Folia primatol., 72: 248–250.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Shultz, S. (2002). Population density, breeding chronology and diet of crowned eagles (Stephanoaetus coronatus) in Taï National Park, Ivory Coast. Ibis, 144: 135–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Shultz, S., Faurie, C., and Noë, R. (2003). Behavioural responses of Diana monkeys to male long-distance calls: Changes in ranging, association patterns and activity. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 53: 238–245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Shultz, S., and Noë, R. (2002). The consequences of crowned eagle central-place foraging on predation risk in monkeys. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B—Biological Sciences, 269: 1797–1802.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Shultz, S., Noë, R., McGraw, W.S., and Dunbar, R.I.M. (2004). AA community-level evaluation of the impact of prey behavioural and ecological characteristics on predator diet composition. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B—Biological Sciences, 271: 725–732.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Shultz, S., and Thomsett, S. (2007). Interactions between African crowned eagles and their primate prey community. In W.S. McGraw, K. Zuberbühler and R. Noë (Eds.), Monkeys of the Taï forest: An African monkey community. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press.Google Scholar
  57. Skorupa, J.P. (1989). Crowned eagles (Stephanoaetus coronatus) in rainforest: Observations on breeding chronology and diet at a nest in Uganda. Ibis, 131: 294–298.Google Scholar
  58. Slocombe, K.E., and Zuberbühler, K. (2005). Agonistic screams in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) vary as a function of social role. J. Comp. Psych., 119: 67–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Stanford, C.B. (1998). Predation and male bonds in primate societies. Behaviour, 135: 513–533.Google Scholar
  60. Struhsaker, T.T., and Leakey, M. (1990). Prey selectivity by crowned hawk-eagles on monkeys in the Kibale Forest, Uganda. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 26: 435–444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Tomasello, M., and Call, J. (1997). Primate cognition. New York: Oxford Univ. Press.Google Scholar
  62. Treves, A. (1999). Has predation shaped the social systems of arboreal primates? International Journal of Primatology, 20: 35–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Treves, A. (2000). Theory and method in studies of vigilance and aggregation. Animal Behaviour, 60: 711–722.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Uehara, S. (1997). Predation on mammals by the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes). Primates, 38: 193–214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. van Schaik, C.P. (1983). Why are diurnal primates living in groups? Behaviour, 87: 120–144.Google Scholar
  66. Wachter, B., Schabel, M., and Noë, R. (1997). DDiet overlap and polyspecific associations of red colobus and Diana monkeys in the Taï Park, Ivory Coast. Ethology, 103: 514–526CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Waser, P.M. (1982). Primate polyspecific associations: Do they occur by chance? Animal Behaviour, 30: 1–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Wolters, S., and Zuberbühler, K. (2003). Mixed-species associations of Diana and Campbell’s monkeys: The costs and benefits of a forest phenomenon. Behaviour, 140: 371–385.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Wright, P. (1998). Impact of predation risk on the behaviour of Propithecus diadema edwardsi in the rainforest of Madagascar. Behaviour, 135: 483–512.Google Scholar
  70. Zuberbühler, K. (2000). Causal cognition in a non-human primate: Field playback experiments with Diana monkeys. Cognition, 76: 195–207.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Zuberbühler, K. (2000). Causal knowledge of predators’ behaviour in wild Diana monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 59: 209–220PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Zuberbühler, K. (2000). Interspecific semantic communication in two forest monkeys. Proc R. Soc. London B, 267: 713–718.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Zuberbühler, K. (2000). Referential labelling in Diana monkeys. Animal Behaviour, 59: 917–927.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Zuberbühler, K. (2001). Predator-specific alarm calls in Campbell’s guenons. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol., 50: 414–422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Zuberbühler, K. (2002). A syntactic rule in forest monkey communication. Animal Behaviour, 63: 293–299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Zuberbühler, K., Cheney, D.L., and Seyfarth, R.M. (1999). Conceptual semantics in a nonhuman primate. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 113: 33–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Zuberbühler, K., and Jenny, D. (2002). Leopard predation and primate evolution. Journal of Human Evolution, 43: 873–886.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Zuberbühler, K., Jenny, D., and Bshary, R. (1999). The predator deterrence function of primate alarm calls. Ethology, 105: 477–490.Google Scholar
  79. Zuberbühler, K., Noë, R., and Seyfarth, R.M. (1997). Diana monkey long-distance calls: Messages for conspecifics and predators. Animal Behaviour, 53: 589–604CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2007

Authors and Affiliations

  • Klaus Zuberbühler
    • 1
  1. 1.School of PsychologyUniversity of St. AndrewsSt. Andrews FifeScotland UK

Personalised recommendations