Skip to main content

Change Blindness: Implications for the Nature of Visual Attention

  • Chapter

Abstract

In the not-too-distant past, vision was often said to involve three levels of processing: a low level concerned with descriptions of the geometric and photometric properties of the image, a high level concerned with abstract knowledge of the physical and semantic properties of the world, and a middle level concerned with anything not handled by the other two.1

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Ballard, D. H. (1991). Animate vision. Artif. Intell., 48: 57–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bridgeman, B., Hendry, D., and Stark, L. (1975). Failure to detect displacement of the visual world during saccadic eye movements. Vis. Res., 15: 719–722.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Grimes, J. (1996). On the failure to detect changes in scenes across saccades. In K. Akins (Ed.), Perception (Vancouver Studies in Cognitive Science, vol. 5, pp. 89–109 ). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, J. M. (1992). Object identification in context: The visual processing of natural scenes. Can. J. Psych., 46, 319–341.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, J. M. and Hollingworth, A. (1999) High-level scene perception. Ann. Rev. Psych., 50: 243–271.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., Treisman, A., and Gibbs, B. (1992). The reviewing of object files: Object-specific integration of information. Cog. Psych., 24: 175–219.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Klein, R., Kingstone, A., and Pontefract, A. (1992). Orienting of visual attention.In K. Rayner (Ed.), Eye Movements and Visual Cognition: Scene Perception and Reading, pp. 46–65. New York, NY: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin, D. T., and Simons, D. J. (1997). Failure to detect changes to attended objects in motion pictures. Psychon. Bull. Rev., 4: 501–506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Man, D. (1982). Vision. San Francisco: Freeman. 9. Change Blindness 187

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore, C. M, and Egeth, H. (1997). Perception without attention: Evidence of grouping under conditions of inattention. J. Exp. Psych.: Human Percept. and Perform., 23, 339–352.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • O’Regan, J. K, Deubel, H., Clark, J. J., and Rensink, R. A. (2000). Picture changes during blinks: Looking without seeing and seeing without looking. Vis. Cog., 7: 191–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pashler, H. (1988). Familiarity and visual change detection. Percept. and Psychophys., 44: 369–378.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pylyshyn, Z. W., and Storm, R. W. (1988). Tracking multiple independent targets: Evidence for a parallel tracking mechanism. Spatial Vis., 3: 179–197.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rensink, R. A. (1998). Limits to attentional selection for orientation. Percept., 27 (suppl.): 36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rensink, R. A. (1999). The magical number one, plus or minus zero. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci., 40: 52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rensink, R. A. (2000a). The dynamic representation of scenes. Vis. Cog., 7: 17–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rensink, R. A. (2000b). Visual search for change: A probe into the nature of attentional processing. Vis. Cog., 7: 345–376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rensink, R. A. (2000c). Seeing, sensing, and scrutinizing. Vis. Res., 40: 1469 1487.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rensink, R. A., and Enns, J. T. (1995). Preemption effects in visual search: Evidence for low-level grouping. Psychol. Rev., 102: 101–130.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rensink, R. A., and Enns, J. T. (1998). Early completion of occluded objects. Vis. Res., 38: 2489–2505.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rensink, R. A., O’Regan, J. K., and Clark, J. J. (1997). To see or not to see: The need for attention to perceive changes in scenes. Psychol. Sci., 8: 368–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rensink, R. A., O’Regan, J. K., and Clark, J. J. (2000). On the failure to detect changes in scenes across brief interruptions. Vis. Cog., 7: 127–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simons, D. J., and Levin, D. T. (1997). Change blindness. Trends Cog. Sci., 1: 261–267.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Simons, D. J., and Levin, D. T. (1998). Failure to detect changes to people during a real-world interaction. Psych. Bull. Rev., 5: 644–649.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stroud, J. M. (1955). The fine structure of psychological time. In H. Quastler (Ed.), Information Theory in Psychology: Problems and Methods. pp. 174207. Glencoe, IL: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A., and Gormican, S. (1988). Feature analysis in early vision: Evidence from search asymmetries. Psychol. Rev., 95: 15–48.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ullman, S. (1996). High-level Vision. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 188 Ronald A. Rensink

    Google Scholar 

  • Ward, R., Duncan, J., and Shapiro, K. (1996). The slow time-course of visual attention. Cog. Psych., 30: 79–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilken, P., Mattingley, J. B., Korb, K. B., Webster, W. R. and Conway, D. (1999). Capacity limits for detection versus reportability of change in visual scenes. Paper presented at the 26th Annual Australian Experimental Psychology Conference, April, 1999.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolfe, J. M. (1999). Inattentional amnesia. In V. Coltheart (Ed.), Fleeting Memories. pp. 71–94. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodhouse, J. M., and Barlow, H. B. (1982). Spatial and temporal resolution and analysis. In H. B. Barlow and J. D. Mollon (Eds.), The Senses. pp. 133–164. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2001 R. Rensink

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Rensink, R.A. (2001). Change Blindness: Implications for the Nature of Visual Attention. In: Jenkin, M., Harris, L. (eds) Vision and Attention. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-21591-4_9

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-21591-4_9

  • Publisher Name: Springer, New York, NY

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4684-9520-1

  • Online ISBN: 978-0-387-21591-4

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics