Skip to main content

Shifts of Attention and Saccades Are Very Similar. Are They Causally Linked?

  • Chapter

Abstract

Attention is first and foremost the servant of cognition, choosing one object among the many available, to paraphrase William James. In addition, attention has a motoric function in selecting the object to be grasped by eye or hand. In this chapter we will first review some of the literature dealing with how visual attention is shifted about and what role it might play in the control of saccadic eye movements, and then briefly describe some experiments of our own on these matters.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Bashinski, H. S. and Bacharach, V. R. (1980). Enhancement of perceptual sensitivity as the result of selectively attending to spatial locations. Percept. Psychophys., 28: 241–248.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brefczynski, J. A. and DeYoe, E. A. (1999). A physiological correlate of the `spotlight’ of visual attention. Nat. Neurosci., 2: 370–374.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bushnell, M. C., Goldberg, M. E., and Robinson, D. L. (1981). Behavioral enhancement of visual responses in monkey cerebral cortex. I. Modulation in posterior parietal cortex related to selective visual attention. J. Neurophysiol., 46: 755–772.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cavanagh, P. (1992). Attention-based motion perception. Science, 257:15631565. (1995). Oculomotor activity and visual spatial attention. Behan Brain Res., 71: 81–88.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colby, C. L., Duhamel, J. R. and Goldberg, M. E. (1996). Visual, presaccadic, and cognitive activation of single neurons in monkey lateral intraparietal area. J. Neurophysiol., 76: 2841–2852.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Corbetta, M. (1998). Frontoparietal cortical networks for directing attention and the eye to visual locations: identical, independent, or overlapping neural systems? Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. USA, 95: 831–838.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Coull, J. T., Frith, C. D., Buchel, C. and Nobre, A. C. (2000). Orienting attention in time: behavioural and neuroanatomical distinction between exogenous and endogenous shifts. Neuropsychologia, 38: 808–819.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Deubel, H. (1995). Separate adaptive mechanisms for the control of reactive and volitional saccadic eye movements. Vis. Res., 35: 3529–3540.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Deubel, H. and Schneider, W. X. (1996). Saccade target selection and object recognition: evidence for a common attentional mechanism. Vis. Res., 36: 1827–1837.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Deubel, H., Wolf, W. and Hauske, G. (1984). The evaluation of the oculomotor error signal. In A. G. Gale and F. Johnson (Eds), Theoretical and Applied Aspects of Eye Movement Research. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Downing, C. J. (1988). Expectancy and visual-spatial attention: effects on perceptual quality. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perf., 14: 188–202.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Downing, P. E. and Treisman, A. M. (1997). The line-motion illusion: attention or impletion? J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform., 23: 768–779.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Egeth, H. E. and Yantis, S. (1997). Visual attention: control, representation, and time course. Ann. Rev. Psychol., 48: 269–297.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Egly, R. and Homa, D. (1991). Reallocation of visual attention. J. Exp. Psych. Human Percept. Perf., 17: 142–159.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, C. W. (1990). Attentional search of the visual field. In D. Brogan (Ed.) Visual Search, 221–240. London: Taylor Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, C. W. and Hoffman, J. E. (1972). Temporal and spatial characteristics of selective encoding from visual displays. Percept. Psychophys., 12: 201–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, C. W. and Hoffman, J. E. (1973). The extent of processing of noise elements during selective encoding from visual displays. Percept. Psychophys., 14: 155–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, C. W. and Murphy, T. D. (1987). Movement of attentional focus across the visual field: a critical look at the evidence. Percept. Psychophys., 42: 299305.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, C. W. and St. James, J. D. (1986). Visual attention within and around the field of focal attention: a zoom lens model. Percept. Psychophys., 40: 225240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, C. W. and Yeh, Y. Y. (1985). Allocation of attention in the visual field. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perf, 11: 583–597.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Erkelens, C. J. and Hulleman, J. (1993). Selective adaptation of internally trig- gered saccades made to visual targets. Exp. Brain Res., 93: 157–164.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, B. and Breitmeyer, B. (1987). Mechanisms of visual attention revealed by saccadic eye movements. Neuropsychologia, 25: 73–83.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, B. and Weber, H. (1988). Significance of attentive fixation for the selection of saccade targets in different parts of the visual field of the rhesus monkey. Exp. Brain Res., 73: 577–582.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, B. W. (1993). Express saccades and visual attention. Behay. Brain Sci., 16: 553–610.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • von Helmholtz, H. (1910). Treatise on Physiological Optics (trans. 1925 from the third German edition, J. P. C. Southall, Ed). New York: Dover (1962).

    Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, J. M. (1992). Visual attention and eye movement control during reading and scene perception. In K. Rayner (Ed.) Eye Movements and Visual Cognition, 260–283. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hikosaka, O., Miyauchi, S. and Shimojo, S. (1993). Focal visual attention produces illusory temporal order and motion sensation. Vis. Res., 33: 1219–1240.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hikosaka, O., Miyauchi, S. and Shimojo, S. (1996). Orienting of spatial attention<its relexive, compensatory and voluntary mechanisms. Cog. Brain Res., 5: 1–9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, J. E. (1975). Hierarchical stages in the processing of visual information. Percept. Psychophys., 18: 348–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, J. E. and Subramaniam, B. (1995). The role of visual attention in saccadic eye movements. Percept. Psychophys., 57: 787–795.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, H. C. and Zimba, L. D. (1985). Spatial maps of directed visual attention. J. Exp. Psych. Human Percept. Perf, 11: 409–430.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, H. C. and Zimba, L. D. (1987). Natural boundaries for the spatial spread of directed visual attention. Neuropsychologia, 25: 5–18.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, G. W. (1981a). Flexibility of attention between stimulus dimensions. Percept. Psychophys., 30: 291–302.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, G. W. (198 lb). On varying the span of visual attention: Evidence for two modes of spatial attention. Q. J. Exp. Psych. A, Human Exp. Psych., 33A: 17–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Husain, M. A. K., (1996). The role of attention in human oculomotor control. In W. H. Zangemeister, H. S. Stiehl and C. Freksa (Eds.), Visual Attention and Cognition, 116: 165–175. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1890). The Principles of Psychology, New York: Holt.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Jonides, J. (1981). Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind’s eye movements. In A. D. Long (Ed.), Attention and Performance IX, 187–203, Hillsadale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jonides, J. (1983). Further toward a model of the mind’s eye’s movement. Bull. Psychon. Soc., 21: 247–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kastner, S., Pinsk, M. A., De Weerd, P., Desimone, R. d Ungerleider, L. G. (1999). Increased activity in human visual cortex during directed attention in the absence of visual stimulation. Neuron., 22: 751–761.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kinchla, R. A. (1992). Attention. Ann. Rev. Psychol., 43: 711–742.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kingstone, A. and Klein, R. M. (1993). Visual offsets facilitate saccadic latency: does predisengagement of visuospatial attention mediate this gap effect? J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perf., 19: 1251–1265.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Klein, R. M. (1980). Does oculomotor readiness mediate cognitive control of visual attention? in ( R. Nickerson, ed.), Attention and Performance, Vol. VIII. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, R., Kingston, A. and Pontefract, A. (1992). Orientating of visual attention. In K. Rayner (Ed.) Eye Movements and Visual Cognition, pp. 46–65, New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Koch, C. and Ullman, S. (1985). Shifts in selective visual attention: towards the underlying neural circuitry. Hum. Neurobiol., 4: 219–227.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kowler, E., Anderson, E., Dosher, B. and Blaser, E. (1995). The role of attention in the programming of saccades. Vis. Res., 35: 1897–1916.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kustov, A. A. and Robinson, D. L. (1996). Shared neural control of attentional shifts and eye movements. Nature, 384: 74–77.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kwak, H. W., Dagenbach, D. and Egeth, H. (1991). Further evidence for a time-independent shift of the focus of attention. Percept. Psychophys., 49: 473480.

    Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, D. (1983). Spatial extent of attention to letters and words. J. Exp. Psychol. Human Percept. Pei f., 9:371–379.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lu, Z. L. and Dosher, B. A. (1998). External noise distinguishes attention mechanisms. Vis. Res., 38: 1183–1198.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Luck, S. J., Chelazzi, L., Hillyard, S. A. and Desimone, R. (1997). Neural mechanisms of spatial selective attention in areas V1, V2, and V4 of macaque visual cortex. J. Neurophysiol., 77: 24–42.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Maljkovic, V. (1999). Spatial scale of attentional deployment changes during saccade preparation. Invest. Ophthal. Vis. Sci., 40: S415.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayfrank, L., Mobashery, M., Kimmig, H. and Fischer, B. (1986). The role of fixation and visual attention in the occurrence of express saccades in man. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Neurol. Sci., 235: 269–275.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McLaughlin, S. (1967). Parametric adjustment in saccadic eye movements. Percept. Psychophys., 2: 359–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McPeek, R. M., Maljkovic, V. and Nakayama, K. (1999). Saccades require focal attention and are facilitated by a short-term memory system. Vis. Res., 39: 1555–1566.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J., Anstis, T. and Templeton, W. (1981). Saccadic plasticity: Parametric adaptive control by retinal feedback. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perf., 7: 356–366.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Müller, H. J., and Findlay, J. M. (1987). Sensitivity and criterion effects in the spatial cuing of visual attention. Percept. Psychophys., 42: 383–399.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Müller, H. J. and Rabbitt, P. M. (1989). Reflexive and voluntary orienting of visual attention: time course of activation and resistance to interruption. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perf., 15: 315–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nakayama, K. and Mackeben, M. (1989). Sustained and transient components of focal visual attention. Vis. Res., 29: 1631–1647.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Niebur, E. and Koch, C. (1994). A model for the neuronal implementation of selective visual attention based on temporal correlation among neurons. J. Comput. Neurosci., 1: 141–158.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Optican, L. M. (1985). Adaptive properties of the saccadic system. In A. M. J.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berthoz (Ed.) Adaptive mechanisms in gaze control: Facts and theories, Vol.1,pp. 71–79. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Q. J. Exp. Psychol., 32: 3–25.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. Snyder, C. R. and Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. J. Exp. Psychol., 109: 160–174.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. L. and Cohen, Y. (1984). Components of visual orienting. In H. Bouma and D. Bouwhuis (Eds.) Attention and Performance X, pp. 531–556. London: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Remington, R. (1980). Attention and saccadic eye movements. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perf, 6: 726–744.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Remington, R. and Pierce, L. (1984). Moving attention: Evidence for time-invariant shifts of visual selective attention. Percept. Psychophys., 35: 393399.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G. (1983). Mechanisms of selective attention in mammals. In J. P. E-wart, R. R. Capranica and D. J. Ingle (Eds.) Advances in Vertebrate Neuroethology, pp. 261–297. New York: Plenum.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., Riggio, L., Dascola, I. and Umilta, C. (1987). Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence. Neuropsychologia, 25: 31–40.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, D. L., Bowman, E. M. and Kertzman, C. (1995). Covert orienting of attention in macaques. II. Contributions of parietal cortex. J. Neurophysiol., 74: 698–712.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sagi, D. and Julesz, B. (1985). Fast noninertial shifts of attention. Spat. Vis., 1: 141–149.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Saslow, M. G. (1967). Effects of components of displacement-step stimuli upon latency for saccadic eye movement. J. Opt. Soc. Am., 57: 1024–1029.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Scudder, C. A., Batourina, E. Y. and Tunder, G. S. (1998). Comparison of two methods of producing adaptation of saccade size and implications for the site of plasticity. J. Neurophysiol., 79: 704–715.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Semmlow, J., Gauthier, G. and Vercher, J.-L. (1989). Mechanisms of short-term saccadic adaptation. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Pei f., 15: 249–258.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sheliga, B. M., Riggio, L., Craighero, L. and Rizzolatti, G. (1995). Spatial attention-determined modifications in saccade trajectories. Neuroreport, 6: 585–588.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sheliga, B. M., Riggio, L. and Rizzolatti, G. (1994). Orienting of attention and eye movements. Exp. Brain Res., 98: 507–522.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shepherd, M., Findlay, J. M. and Hockey, G. R. J. (1986). The relationship between eye movements and spatial attention. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A, 38: 475491.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepherd, M. and Müller, H. J. (1989). Movement versus focusing of visual attention. Percept. and Psychophys., 46: 146–154.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shimojo, S., Miyauchi, S. and Hikosaka, O. (1997). Visual motion sensation yielded by non-visually driven attention. Vis. Res., 37: 1575–1580.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shulman, G. L., Remington, R. W. and McLean, J. P. (1979). Moving attention through visual space. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Pei 1., 5: 522–526.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sperling, G. and Weichselgartner, E. (1995). Episodic theory of the dynamics of spatial attention. Psychol. Rev., 102: 503–532.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steinman, B. A., Steinman, S. B. and Lehmkuhle, S. (1995). Visual attention mechanisms show a center-surround organization. Vis. Res., 35: 1859–1869.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Steinmetz, P. N., Roy, A., Fitzgerald, P. J., Hsiao, S. S., Johnson, K. O. and Niebur, E. (2000). Attention modulates synchronized neuronal firing in primate somatosensory cortex. Nature, 404: 187–190.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tam, W. J. and Stelmach, L. B. (1993). Viewing behavior: ocular and attentional disengagement. Percept. Psychophys., 54: 211–22.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A. M. (1977). Focused attention in the perception and retrieval of multidimensional stimuli. Percept. Psychophys., 22: 1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A. M. (1982). Perceptual grouping and attention in visual search for features and for objects. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perf, 8: 194–214.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A. M. (1988). Features and objects: the fourteenth Bartlett memorial lecture. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. A, 40: 201–237.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Treisman, A. M., and Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognit. Psychol., 12: 97–136.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Treue, S. and Maunsell, J. H. (1999). Effects of attention on the processing of motion in macaque middle temporal and medial superior temporal visual cortical areas. J. Neurosci., 19: 7591–7602.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Tsal, Y. (1983). Movements of attention across the visual field. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Pelf, 9: 523–530.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wallman, J. and Fuchs, A. F. (1998). Saccadic gain modification: visual error drives motor adaptation. J. Neurophysiol., 80: 2405–2416.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wundt, W. (1912). Introduction to Psychology. London: Allen.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wurtz, R. H. and Mohler, C. W. (1976). Organization of monkey superior colliculus: enhanced visual response of superficial layer cells. J. Neurophysiol., 39: 745–765.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Yantis, S. (1988). On analog movements of visual attention. Percept. Psychophys. 43: 203–206.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Yantis, S. and Jonides, J. (1990). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: voluntary versus automatic allocation. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perf. 16: 121–134.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2001 Springer Science+Business Media New York

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

McFadden, S., Wallman, J. (2001). Shifts of Attention and Saccades Are Very Similar. Are They Causally Linked?. In: Jenkin, M., Harris, L. (eds) Vision and Attention. Springer, New York, NY. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-21591-4_2

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-21591-4_2

  • Publisher Name: Springer, New York, NY

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-4684-9520-1

  • Online ISBN: 978-0-387-21591-4

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics