Skip to main content

Spatial Frames of Reference Used in Identifying Direction of Movement: An Unexpected Turn

  • Conference paper
  • First Online:
Book cover Spatial Information Theory (COSIT 2001)

Part of the book series: Lecture Notes in Computer Science ((LNCS,volume 2205))

Included in the following conference series:

Abstract

Despite extensive interest in the role of frames of reference in spatial representation, there is little consensus regarding the cognitive effort associated with various reference systems and the cognitive costs (if any) involved in switching from one frame of reference to another. Relevant to these issues an experiment was conducted in which accuracy and response latency data were collected in a task in which observers verified the direction of turns made by a model car in a mock city in terms of four different spatial frames of reference: fixed-observer (relative-egocentric), fixed-environmental object (intrinsic-fixed), mobile object (intrinsic-mobile), and cardinal directions (absolute-global). Results showed that frames of reference could be differentiated on the basis of response accuracy and latency. In addition, no cognitive costs were observed in terms of accuracy or latency when the frames of reference switched between fixed-observer vs. global frames of reference or between mobile object and fixed environmental object frames of reference. Instead, a distinct performance advantage was observed when frames of reference were changed. This unexpected result is attributed to a phenomenon analogous to release from proactive inhibition.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Acredolo, L. P. (1976). Frames of reference used by children for orientation in unfamiliar spaces. In G. Moore & R. Golledge (Eds.), Environmental Knowing (pp. 165–172). Stroudsburg, PA: Dowden, Hutchinson, & Ross.

    Google Scholar 

  • Acredolo, L. P. (1990). Behavioral approaches to spatial orientation in infancy. In A. Diamond (Ed.), The development and neural bases of higher cognitive function (pp. 596–607). New York: New York Academy of Sciences.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allen, G. L. (1999). Children’s control of reference systems in spatial tasks: Foundations of spatial cognitive skill? Spatial Cognition and Computation, 1, 413–429.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bremner, G. J. (1978). Egocentric versus allocentric spatial coding in nine-monthold infants: Factors influencing the choice of code. Developmental Psychology, 14, 346–355.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bremner, G. J., Knowles, L., & Andreasen, G. (1994). Processes underlying young children’s spatial orientation during movement. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 57, 355–376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, D. J. & Tversky, B. (1999). Mental representations of perspective and spatial relations from diagrams and models. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 137–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson-Radvansky, L. A. & Jiang, Y. (1998). Inhibition accompanies reference-frame selection. Psychological Science, 9, 386–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cornell, E. H., & Heth, C. D. (1979). Response versus place learning by human infants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 5, 188–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frank, A. U. (1998). Formal models for cognition-Taxonomy of spatial location description and frames of reference. In C. Freksa, C. Habel, & K. F. Wender (Eds.), Spatial cognition: An interdisciplinary approach to representing and processing spatial knowledge (pp. 293–312). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Franklin, N., Tversky, B., & Coon, V. (1992). Switching points of view in spatial mental models. Memory and Cognition, 20, 507–518.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heth, C. D., & Cornell, E. H. (1980). Three experiences affecting spatial discrimination learning by ambulatory children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 30, 246–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klatsky, R. L. (1998). Allocentric and egocentric spatial representations: Definitions, distinctions, and interconnections. In C. Freksa, C. Habel, & K. F. Wender (Eds.), Spatial cognition: An interdisciplinary approach to representing and processing spatial knowledge (pp. 1–17). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W. J. M. (1996). Perspective taking ellipsis in spatial descriptions. In P. Bloom, M. A. Peterson, L. Nadel, & M. F. Garrett (Eds.), Language and space (pp. 77–107). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, S. C. (1996). Frames of reference and Molyneux’s question: Crosslinguistic evidence. In P. Bloom, M. A. Peterson, L. Nadel, & M. F. Garrett (Eds.), Language and space (pp. 109–169). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (1995). Linguistic and conceptual control of visual spatial attention. Cognitive Psychology, 28, 103–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (1996). Top-down control of reference frame alignment in direction attention from cue to target. In A. F. Kramer, M. G. H. Coles, & G. D. Logan (Eds.), Converging operations in the study of visual selective attention (pp 415-437). Washington, D. C: APA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newcombe, N. S., & Huttenlocher, J. (2000). Making space: The development of spatial representation and reasoning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rieser, J. J. (1979). Spatial orientation in six-month-old infants. Child Development, 50, 1078–1087.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, H. A., & Tversky, B. (1992). Descriptions and depictions of environments. Memory & Cognition, 20, 483–496.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, H. A. & Tversky, B. (1996). Perspective in spatial descriptions. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 371–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B. (1991). Spatial mental models. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (pp. 109–145). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B., Lee, P., & Mainwaring, S. (1999). Why do speakers mix perspectives? Spatial Cognition and Computation, 1, 399–412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2001 Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg

About this paper

Cite this paper

Miller, C.R., Allen, G.L. (2001). Spatial Frames of Reference Used in Identifying Direction of Movement: An Unexpected Turn. In: Montello, D.R. (eds) Spatial Information Theory. COSIT 2001. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, vol 2205. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg. https://doi.org/10.1007/3-540-45424-1_14

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/3-540-45424-1_14

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg

  • Print ISBN: 978-3-540-42613-4

  • Online ISBN: 978-3-540-45424-3

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics