Skip to main content

Sociocultural Contexts of Learning

  • Chapter

Part of the book series: International and Cultural Psychology Series ((ICUP))

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD   109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Adamson, L. B., and Bakeman, R. (1991). The development of shared attention during infancy. In: Vasta, R. (ed.), Annals of child development, Volume 8. Kingsley, London, pp. 1–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ashley, J., and Tomasello, M. (1998). Cooperative problem solving and teaching in preschoolers. Social Development 17:143–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Azmitia, M. (1988). Peer interaction and problem solving, When are two heads better than one? Child Development 59:87–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bakeman, R., Adamson, L. B., Konner, M., and Barr, R. G. (1990). !Kung infancy, The social context of object exploration. Child Development 61:794–809.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1962). Social learning through imitation. In: Jones, M. R. (ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation, Volume 10. University of Nebraska Press, Lincoln, NE, pp. 211–271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action, A social cognitive theory. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barr, R., Dowden, A., and Hayne, H. (1996). Developmental changes in deferred imitation by 6-to 24-month-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development 19:159–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, M. H., Tal, J., and Tamis-LeMonda, C. (1991). Parenting in cross-cultural perspective. In: Bornstein, M. H. (ed.), Cultural approaches to parenting. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 69–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bransford, J., Brown, A. L., and Cocking, R. R. (1999). How people learn, Brain, mind, experience, and school. National Academy Press, Washington, DC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brownell, C. A., and Carriger, M. S. (1991). Collaborations among toddler peers, Individual contributions to social contexts. In: Resnick, L. B., Levine, J. M., and Teasley, S. D. (eds.), Perspectives on socially shared cognition. American Psychological Association, Washington, DC, pp. 365–383.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1975). The ontogenesis of speech acts. Journal of Child Language. 1:1–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1995). From joint attention to the meeting of minds, An introduction. In: Moore, C., and Dunham, P. J. (eds.), Joint attention, Its origins and role in development, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 1–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campos, J. J., and Stenberg, C. R. (1981). Perception, appraisal, and emotion: The onset of social referencing. In: Lamb, M. E., and Sherrod, L. R. (eds.), Infant social cognition, Empirical and theoretical considerations, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 273–314.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, M., Nagall, K., and Tomasello, M. (1998). Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63(Serial No. 255). University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chavajay, P., and Rogoff, B. (1999). Cultural variation in the management of attention by children and their caregivers. Child Development 35:1079–1090.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M. (1996). Cultural psychology, A once and future discipline. Harvard University Press Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crick, N. R., and Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social-information processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin 115:74–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ding, S., and Flynn, E. (2000). Collaborative learning, An underlying skills approach. In: Joiner, R., Littleton, K., Faulkner, D., and Miell, D. (eds.), Rethinking collaborative learning, Free Association Books, London, pp. 3–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doise, W., Mugny, G., and Perret-Clermont, A. (1975). Social interaction and the development of cognitive operations. European Journal of Social Psychology 5:367–383.

    Google Scholar 

  • Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the modern mind: Three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duran, R. T., and Gauvain, M. (1993). The role of age versus expertise in peer collaboration during joint planning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 55:227–242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, S., and Gauvain, M. (1992). Social and cultural influences on children’s collaborative interactions. In: Winegar, L. T., and J. Valsiner, J. (eds.), Children’s development within social context, Volume 2, Research and methodology, Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum, pp. 155–180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fivush, R., Haden, C., and Reese, E. (1996). Remembering, recounting, and reminiscing, The development of autobiographical memory in social context. In: Rubin, D. C. (ed.), Remembering our past, Studies in autobiographical memory, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 341–359.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gauvain, M. (1995). Thinking in niches, Sociocultural influences on cognitive development. Human Development 38:25–45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gauvain, M. (2001). The social context of cognitive development. Guilford, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gauvain, M., and Rogoff, B. (1989). Collaborative problem solving and children’s planning skills. Developmental Psychology 25:139–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glachen, M., and Light, P. (1982). Peer interaction and learning, Can two wrongs make a right? In: Butterworth, G., and Light, P. (eds.), Social cognition, Studies of the development of understanding, University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp. 238–262.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodnow, J. J. (1990). The socialization of cognition, What’s involved? In, Stigler, J. W., Shweder, R. A., and G. Herdt, G. (eds.), Cultural psychology, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 259–286.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goody, J. (1977). The domestication of the savage mind. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenfield, P. M. (1999). Cultural change and human development. In: Turiel, E. (ed.), New directions in child development. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, CA, pp. 37–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenfield, P. M., and Childs, C. P. (1991). Developmental continuity in biocultural context. Cohen, R., and Sigel, A. W. (eds.), Context and development, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 135–159.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamond, N. R., and Fivush, R. (1991). Memories of Mickey Mouse, Young children recount their trip to Disneyworld. Cognitive Development 6:433–448.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Helling, G. A. (1966). The Turkish village as a social system. Occidental College, Los Angeles, CA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hopkins, B., and Westra, T. (1990). Motor development, maternal expectations, and the role of handling. Infant Behavior and Development 13:117–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horowitz, F D. (2000). Child development and the PITS, Simple questions, complex answers, and developmental theory. Child Development 71:1–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hudson, J. A. (1990). The emergence of autobiographical memory in mother-child conversation. In Fivush, R., and Hudson, J. A. (eds.), Knowing and remembering in young children, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp. 166–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kagitcibasi, C. (1996). Family and human development across cultures. Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Konner, M. (1975). Relations among infants and toddlers in comparative perspective. In Lewis, M., and Rosenblum, L. A. (eds.), Friendship and peer relations. Wiley, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, D. W., and Johnson, R. T. (1987). Learning together and alone: Cooperative, competitive, and individualistic learning, 2nd. ed. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joiner, R., Littleton, K., Faulkner, D., and Miell, D. (2000). Rethinking collaborative learning. Free Association Books, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laosa, L. (1980). Maternal teaching strategies in Chicano and Anglo-American families: The influence of culture and education on maternal behavior. Child Development 51:759–765.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lave, J., and Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning, Legitimate peripheral participation. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • LeVine, R., Dixon, S., LeVine, S., Richman, A., Leiderman, P. H., Keefer, C. H., and Brazelton, T. B. (1994). Child care and culture, Lessons from Africa. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mackie, D. (1983). The effect of social interaction on conservation of spatial relations. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology 14:131–151.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martini, M. (1994). Balancing work and family in Hawai‘i, Strategies of parents in two cultural groups. Family Perspective 28:103–127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martini, M., and Kirkpatrick, J. (1992). Parenting in Polynesia: A view from the Marquesas. In: Roopnarine, J. L., and Carter, D. B. (eds.), Parent-child socialization in diverse cultures, Volume 5, Annual advances in applied developmental psychology, Ablex, Norwood, NJ, pp. 199–222.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maynard, A. E. (2002). Cultural teaching: The development of teaching skills in Maya sibling interactions. Child Development 73:969–982.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N. (1988). Infant imitation and memory, nine-month-olds in immediate and deferred tests. Child Development 59:217–225.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Middleton, D. (1997). The social organization of conversational remembering: Experience as individual and collective concerns. Mind, Culture, and Activity 4:71–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, P. J., Wiley, A. R., Fung, H., and Liang, C. (1997). Personal storytelling as a medium of socialization in Chinese and American families. Child Development 68:557–568.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mistry, J. (1997). The development of remembering in cultural context. In: Cowan, N. (ed.), The development of memory in childhood, Psychology Press, Hove, East Sussex, UK, pp. 343–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morelli, G. A., Rogoff, B., and Angellilo, C. (2003). Cultural variation in young children’s access to work or involvement in specialised child-focused activities. International Journal of Behavioral Development 27:264–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mullen, M. K., and Yi, S. (1995). The cultural context of talk about the past: Implications for the development of autobiographical memory. Cognitive Development 10:407–419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. (1996). Language in cognitive development. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ochs, E., and Schieffelin, B. (1984). Language acquisition and socialization: Three developmental stories. In Shweder, R., and LeVine, R. (eds.), Culture theory, Essays on mind, self and emotion, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp. 276–320.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ochs, E., and Schieffelin, B. (1994). The impact of language socialization on grammatical development. In Fletcher, P., and MacWhinney, B. (eds.), Handbook of child language. Oxford, UK, Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ochs, E., Taylor, C., Rudolph, D., and Smith, R. (1992). Storytelling as a theory-building activity. Discourse Processes 15:37–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palinscar, A. S., and Brown, A. L. (1984). Reciprocal teaching of comprehension monitoring activities. Cognition and Instruction 1:117–175.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1932). The moral judgment of the child. Free Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ratner, H. H. (1980). The role of social context in memory development. In: Perlmutter, M. (ed.), Children’s memory, Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, pp. 49–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ratner, H. H. (1984). Memory demands and the development of young children’s memory. Child Development 55:2173–2191.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development in social context. Oxford University Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B. (1998). Cognition as a collaborative process. In Damon, W. (Series ed.) and Kuhn, D., and Siegler, R. S. (Vol. eds.), Cognition, perception and language, Volume 2, Handbook of child psychology, 5th ed. Wiley, New York, pp. 679–744.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B., Gauvain, M., and Ellis, S. (1984). Development viewed in its cultural context. In Bornstein, M. H., and Lamb, M. E., (eds.), Developmental psychology, Plenum Press, New York, pp. 261–290.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B., Mosier, C., Mistry, J., and Goncu, A. (1993). Guided participation in cultural activity by toddlers and caregivers. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58 (7, Serial No. 236). University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rogoff, B., Paradise, R., Mejía Arauz, R., Correa-Chavez, M., and Angelillo, C. (2003). First-hand learning through intent participation. Annual Review of Psychology 54:175–203.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rommetveit, R. (1985). Language acquisition as increasing linguistic structuring of experience and symbolic behavior control. In J. V. Wertsch (ed.), Culture, communication, and cognition Vygotskian perspectives, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp. 183–204.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruff, H. A., and Rothbart, M. K. (1996). Attention in early development, Themes and variations. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segall, M. H., Dasen, P. R., Berry, J. W., and Poortinga, Y. H. (1999). Human behavior in global perspective: An introduction to cross-cultural psychology, 2nd ed. Allyn and Bacon, Boston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Serpell, R., and Hatano, G. (1997). Education, schooling, and literacy. In: Berry, J. W., Dasen, P. R., and Saraswathi, T. S. (eds.), Handbook of cross-cultural psychology, Volume 2. Basic processes and human development, Allyn and Bacon, Boston, pp. 339–376.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, M. (1996). Conversation and cognition. In Gelman, R., and Au, T. (eds.), Carterette, E. C., and Friedman, P. (Gen. eds.), Handbook of perception and cognition, 2nd ed., Perceptual and cognitive development, Academic Press, San Diego, CA, pp. 243–282.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sigel, I. E., McGillicuddy-DeLisi, A. V., and Goodnow, J. J. (1992). Parental belief systems: The psychological consequences for children, 2nd ed. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S. (2000). The rebirth of children’s learning. Child Development 71:26–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. E. (1990). Building memories: The ontogeny of autobiography. In Cicchetti, D., and Beeghly, M. (eds.), The self in transition, Infancy to childhood, University of Chicago Press, Chicago, pp. 213–242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Super, C. M. (1983). Cultural variation in the meaning and uses of children’s intelligence. In Deregowski, J. G., Dziurawiec, S., and Annis, R. C. (eds.), Explorations in cross-cultural psychology, pp. 199–212. Lisse, Swets and Zeitlinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Super, C. M., and Harkness, S. (1982). The infant’s niche in rural Kenya and metropolitan America. In Adler, L. L. (ed.), Cross-cultural research at issue, Academic Press, San Diego, CA, pp. 47–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Super, C. M., and Harkness, S. (1986). The developmental niche: A conceptualization at the interface of child and culture. International Journal of Behavioral Development 9:545–569.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In Moore, C., and Dunham, P. H., (eds.), Joint attention, Its origins and role in development, Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum, pp. 103–130.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., and Farrar, M. J. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development 57:1454–1463.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., and Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16:495–552.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotksy, L. S. (1978). Mind in society. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, D. J., and Middleton, D. (1975). A study of assisted problem solving. British Journal of Psychology 66:181–191.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2005 Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Gauvain, M. (2005). Sociocultural Contexts of Learning. In: Learning in Cultural Context. International and Cultural Psychology Series. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/0-387-27550-9_2

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics