Skip to main content

Part of the book series: History and Philosophy of Psychology ((HPPS))

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Allport, G.W. (1937). Personality, A psychological interpretation. New York: Holt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Argyris, Chr. (1968). Some unintended consequences of rigorous research. Psychological Bulletin, 70, 183–197.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ash, M.G. (1995). Gestalt psychology in German culture, 1890–1967. Cambridge, Mass.: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bahle, J. (1936). Der musikalische Schaffensprozess; Psychologie der schöpferischen Erlebnis—und Arbeitsformen. Konstanz: Christiani.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, A.L. (1942). Personal structure analysis: A statistical method for investigation of the single personality. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 37, 163–183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barlow, D.H. & Hersen, M. (1984). Single case experimental designs. New York: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartlett, F. (1958). Thinking, an experimental and social study. London: Allen & Unwin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernard, C. (1865/1957). An introduction to the study of experimental medicine (English translation). New York: Dover.

    Google Scholar 

  • Binet, A. & Henneguy, L. (1894). La psychologie des grands calculateurs et joueurs d’échecs. Paris: Flammarion.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bolgar, H. (1965). The case study method. In B.B. Wolman (Ed.), Handbook of clinical psychology (pp. 28–39). New York: McGraw Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boring, E.G. (1943). The moon illusion. American Journal of Physics, 11, 55–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boring, E.G. (1950). A history of experimental psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boring, E.G. (1953). A history of introspection. Psychological Bulletin, 50, 169–189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, D.T. & Stanley, J.C. (1963). Experimental and quasi-experimental design for research. Chicago: Rand McNally.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chassan, J.B. (1979). Research design in clinical psychology and psychiatry. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Claparède, E. (1917). Psychologie de l’intelligence. Scientia, 22, 353–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • Claparède, E. (1932). Genèse de l’hypothèse. Archives de Psychologie, 24, 1–155.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, T.D. & Campbell, D.T. (Eds.) (1979). Quasi-experimentation: Design and analysis issues for field settings. Chicago: Rand McNally.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A.R. (1994). Descartes’ error; Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Putnam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Danziger, K. (1985). The origins of the psychological experiment as a social institution. American Psychologist, 40, 133–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Danziger, K. (1987). Statistical method and the historical development of research practice in American psychology. In L. Krüger et al. (Eds.), The probabilistic revolution Vol. II (pp. 35–47). Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Danziger, K. (1990). Constructing the subject; historical origins of psychological research. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, P.O. & Costello, C.G. (1969). N = 1: Experimental studies of single cases. New York: van Nostrand.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehue, T. (1995). Changing the rules; Psychology in the Netherlands, 1900–1985. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehue, T. (1997). Deception, efficiency, and random groups; Psychology and the gradual origination of the random group design. Isis, 88, 653–673.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehue, T. (2000). From deception trials to control reagents; The introduction of the control group about a century ago. American Psychologist, 55, 264–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dehue, T. (2001). Establishing the experimenting society: The historical origin of social experimentation according to the randomized controlled design. American Journal of Psychology, 114, 283–302.

    Google Scholar 

  • Donders, F.C. (1868/1969). On the speed of mental processes (English translation). Acta Psychologica, 30, (Special Issue on Attention and Performance), 412–431.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duncker, K. (1926). A qualitative (experimental and theoretical) study of productive thinking (solving of comprehensible problems). Pedagogical Seminary, 33, 642–708.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duncker, K. (1935). Zur Psychologie des produktiven Denkens. Berlin: Julius Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K.A. & Simon, H.A. (1984). Protocol analysis; Verbal reports as data. Cambridge Mass.: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ericsson, K.A. & Smith, J. (Eds.) (1991). Toward a general theory of expertise. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H.J. (1952). The scientific study of personality. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fechner, G.Th. (1876). Vorschule der Aesthetik. Leipzig: Breitkopf und Härtel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fechner, G.Th. (1897). Kollektivmaßlehre. Leipzig: Engelmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feigenbaum, E.A. (1984). Knowledge engineering; the applied side of artificial intelligence. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, Vol. 426 (Special Issue: Computer Culture), 97–107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G. (1987). Survival of the fittest probabilist: Brunswik, Thurstone, and the two disciples of psychology. In: Krüger, L., Gigerenzer, G. & Morgan, M. (Eds.) (1987). The probabilistic revolution II: Ideas in the sciences (pp. 49–72). Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G. et al. (1989). The empire of chance; how probability changed science and everyday life. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gigerenzer, G. & Murray, D.J. (1987). Cognition as intuitive statistics. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graumann, C. (1952). Die Kriterien des Einfallserlebnis (Inauguraldissertation Köln), Ed. 1955.

    Google Scholar 

  • Groot, A.D. de (1946/1965). Thought and choice in chess. (English translation) Den Haag: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Groot, A.D. de (1969). Methodology; Foundations of inference and research in the behavioral sciences. The Hague: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Groot, A.D. de & Gobet, F. (1996). Perception and memory in chess. Studies in the heuristics of the professional eye. Assen: Van Gorcum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hacking, I. (1975). The emergence of probability. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamel, R. (1990). Over het denken van de architect, een cognitief psychologische beschrijving van het ontwerpproces bij architecten. Amsterdam: AHA-Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heymans, G. (1887). Quantitative Untersuchungen über die Zöllnersche und Loebsche Täuschung. Zeitschrift für Psychologie und Physiologie der Sinnesorgane, 14, 101–139. [Also in: Gesammelte kleinere Schriften II, pp. 35–71. Den Haag: Nijhoff].

    Google Scholar 

  • Heymans, G. (1896). Een laboratorium voor experimenteele psychologie. De Gids, 60, Dl. II, 73–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hilts, V.L. (1973). Statistics and social science. In R.N. Giere & R.S. Westphal (Eds.), Foundations of scientific method (pp. 206–233). Bloomington, Ind.: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holzkamp, K. (1972). Verborgene anthropologische Voraussetzungen der allgemeinen Psychologie. In K. Holzkamp. Kritische Psychologie (pp. 35–73). Frankfurt a. M: Fischer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Honig, W.K. (Ed.) (1966). Operant behavior: Areas of research and application. New York: Appleton Century Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jongman, R.W. (1968). Het oog van de meester, een experimenteel-psychologisch onderzoek naar waarnemingsprestaties van schaakmeesters en ongeoefende schakers. Assen: Van Gorcum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katz, D. (1934). Würdigung G.E. Müller. Acta Psychologica, 1, 234–240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kazdin, A.E. (1982). Single-case design: Methods for clinical and applied settings. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kazdin, A.E. (1992). Research design in clinical psychology. Needham Heihts, Mass.: Allyn & Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kidd, A.L. (Ed.) (1978). Knowledge acquisition for expert systems. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krantz, D.L. (1971). The separate worlds of operant and non-operant psychology. Journal of Applied Behavioral Analysis, 4, 61–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krüger, L., Daston, L. & Heidelberger, M. (Eds.) (1987). The probabilistic revolution I: Ideas in history. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krüger, L., Gigerenzer, G. & Morgan, M. (Eds.) (1987). The probabilistic revolution II: Ideas in the sciences. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, J.M. (1965). On Binocular Rivalry. Assen: Van Gorcum.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGraw, K.L. & Harbison-Briggs, K. (1989). Knowledge acquisition; principles and guidelines. Englewood Cliffs, N. Jersey: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michon, J.A. (1967). Timing in temporal tracking. Assen: Van Gorcum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A., Shaw, J.C. & Simon, H.A. (1958). Elements of a theory of human problem solving. Psychological Review, 65, 151–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A., Shaw, J.C. & Simon, H.A. (1963). Empirical explorations with the logic theory machine: a case study in heuristics. In: E.A. Feigenbaum & J. Feldman (Eds.), Computers and Thought (pp. 109–133). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A. & Simon, H. A. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Orne, M.T. (1962). On the social psychology of the psychological experiment: with particular reference to demand characteristics and their implications. American Psychologist, 17, 776–783.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osgood, Ch.E. & Luria, Z. (1954). A blind analysis of a case of multiple personality using the semantic differential. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 49, 579–591.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pollack, R.H. & Brenner, M.W. (Eds.) (1969). The experimental psychology of Alfred Binet; Selected papers. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Porter, T.M. (1983). Private chaos, public order: The Nineteenth-Century statistical revolution. In M. Heidelberger, L. Krüger & R. Rheinwald (Eds.), Probability since 1800 (pp. 27–40). Bielefeld: Universität Bielefeld; Wissenschaftsforschung/Science studies, Report 25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Porter, T.M. (1986). The rise of statistical thinking 1820-1900. Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosenzweig, S. (1951). Idiodynamics in personality theory with special reference to projective methods. Psychological Review, 38, 213–223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Selz, O. (1913). Ueber die Gesetze des geordneten Denkverlaufs. Eine experimentelle Untersuchung. Stuttgart: Spemann. (English excerpts in: N.H. Frijda en A.D. de Groot (Eds.) (1981), Otto Selz; his contribution to psychology. (pp. 76–146). The Hague: Mouton).

    Google Scholar 

  • Selz, O. (1922). Zur Psychologie des produktiven Denkens und des Irrtums. Bonn: Cohen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, M.B. (1951). An experimental approach to diagnostic psychological testing. Journal of mental science, 97, 748–764.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, M.B. (1957). Experimental method in the psychological description of the individual psychiatric patient. International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 3, 89–102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (1960). Tactics of scientific research; Evaluating experimental data in psychology. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B.F. (1966). Operant behavior. In W.K. Honig (Ed.), Operant behavior: Areas of research and application (pp. 12–32). New York: Appleton Century Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B.F. (1983). A matter of consequences. (Part three of an autobiography). New York: Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snoek, J.W. (1989). Het denken van de neuroloog. Dissertation Rijks-Universiteit Groningen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sprung, H. & Sprung, L. (1988). Gustav Theodor Fechner als experimenteller Ästhetiker—Zur Entwicklung der Methodologie und Methodik einer Psychophysik höherer kognitiver Prozesse. In J. Brozek & H. Gundlach (Eds.), G.T. Fechner and Psychology (pp. 217–227). Passau: Passavia Universitätsverlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stelmach, G.E. (1978). Motor control: issues and trends. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stephenson, W. (1935). Correlating persons instead of tests. Character and Personality, 6, 17–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stephenson, W. (1953). The study of behavior; Q-technique and its methodology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern, W. (1900). Über Psychologie der individuallen Differenzen; Ideen zu einer differentiellen Psychologie. Leipzig: Barth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strien, P.J. van (1991). Audiences, alliances, and the dynamics of science. Transforming Psychology in the Netherlands II. History of the Human Sciences, 4, 351–369.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strien. P.J. van (1993a). The historical practice of theory construction, In: H.V. Rappard, P.J. van Strien, L.P. Mos & W.J. Baker (Eds.) Theory and History; Annals of Theoretical Psychology, Vol. VIII (pp. 149–228). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strien. P.J. van (1993b). Nederlandse psychologen en hun publiek; Een contextuele geschiedenis. Assen: Van Gorcum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strien, P.J. van (1995). Der Experimentierstil in den niederländischen psychologischen Laboratorien. In S. Jaeger et al. (Eds.), Beiträge zur Geschichte der Psychologie (pp. 221–227). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strien, P.J. van (1996). Das Fortbestehen des “Leipziger Modells” in der modernen Psychonomie. In H. Gundlach (Ed.), Untersuchungen zur Geschichte der Psychologie und der Psychotechnik (pp. 105–115). München: Profil.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strien, P.J. van (1997). The American “colonization” of Northwest European social psychology after World War II. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 33, 349–363.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strien, P.J. van (1998). Early applied psychology between essentialism and pragmatism: The dynamics of theory, tools and clients. History of Psychology, 1, 179–204.

    Google Scholar 

  • Titchener, E.B. (1906). Experimental Psychology; A manual of laboratory practice. Vol. I Qualitative experiments, Part 1. Student’s manual. London: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodworth, R.S. (1950). Experimental psychology. London: Methuen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yin, R.K. (1988). Case Study research; design and methods. London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2005 Springer Science + Business Media, Inc.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

van Strien, P.J. (2005). Paris, Leipzig, Danziger, and Beyond. In: Brock, A.C., Louw, J., van Hoorn, W. (eds) Rediscovering the History of Psychology. History and Philosophy of Psychology. Springer, Boston, MA. https://doi.org/10.1007/0-306-48031-X_5

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics