Advertisement

Conversation Among Primate Species

  • Loïc Pougnault
  • Florence Levréro
  • Alban LemassonEmail author
Chapter
  • 37 Downloads

Abstract

The literature in psychology and sociolinguistic suggests that human interlocutors, when conversing, virtually sign a sort of contract that defines the exchange rules in both structural and social domains. These rules make the messages more understandable and the interaction more predictable, but they may also act as a social bond regulator. These rules can be very basic such as speech overlap avoidance, respect of response delays, turn-taking and vocal accommodation to the context and interlocutor’s social status. Interestingly, these rules are universally spread among human cultures questioning their biological basis and motivating the search for possible parallels with our primate cousins. Here, we will review the available literature on monkeys and apes. We will describe the different forms of vocal interactions, the temporal rules underlying these coordinated interactions, the non-random social selection of interlocutors and the context-dependent acoustic plasticity associated to these exchanges. The fact that primate species are socially varied, in terms of both social structure and social organisation, is another interesting aspect, since different social needs may predict different vocal interaction patterns and conversational rules. For example, duets, choruses and dyadic exchanges are not randomly distributed in the primate phylogeny and may even show different functions. Also, age proximity, kin membership, social affinity and hierarchy seem to play species-specific roles. Regarding plasticity, cases of vocal sharing and acoustic matching have been described in some species, notably in contact calls which are the calls the most frequently involved in dyadic exchanges. At last, a few studies also show that these ‘primitive’ conversational rules are often broken by juveniles and that the appropriate way to vocally interact with others may be socially learned, thus another aspect that do not seem strictly human.

Keywords

Vocal accommodation Conversation Turn-taking Vocal interaction 

References

  1. Aitken PG (1981) Cortical control of conditioned and spontaneous vocal behavior in rhesus monkeys. Brain Lang 13(1):171–184.  https://doi.org/10.1016/0093-934X(81)90137-1CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Arlet M, Jubin R, Masataka N, Lemasson A (2015) Grooming-at-a-distance by exchanging calls in non-human primates. Biol Lett 11(10):20150711–20150714.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2015.0711CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Babel M (2010) Dialect divergence and convergence in New Zealand English. Lang Soc 39(4):437–456.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S0047404510000400CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Babel M (2012) Evidence for phonetic and social selectivity in spontaneous phonetic imitation. J Phon 40:177–189.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2011.09.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bailey WJ (2003) Insect duets: underlying mechanisms and their evolution. Physiol Entomol 28:157–174.  https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-3032.2003.00337.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Bascom WR (1942) The principle of seniority in the social structure of the Yoruba. Am Anthropol 44:37–46.  https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.1942.44.1.02a00050CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Beach WA (1990) Searching for universal features of conversation. Res Lang Soc Interact 24(1–4):351–368.  https://doi.org/10.1080/08351819009389347CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Beebe B, Jaffe J, Feldstein S, Mays K, Alson D (1985) Interpersonal timing: the application of an adult dialogue model to mother-infant vocal and kinesic interactions. In: Field T, Fox N (eds) Social perception in infants. Ablex Publishing, Norwood, pp 217–247Google Scholar
  9. Behr O, Knörnschild M, Von Helversen O (2009) Territorial counter-singing in male sac-winged bats saccopteryx bilineata: low-frequency songs trigger a stronger response. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 63(3):433–442.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-008-0677-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Biben M, Symmes D, Masataka N (1986) Temporal and structural analysis of affiliative vocal exchanges in squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus). Behaviour 98(1):259–273Google Scholar
  11. Bouchet H, Pellier AS, Blois-Heulin C, Lemasson A (2010) Sex differences in the vocal repertoire of adult red-capped mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus): a multi-level acoustic analysis. Am J Primatol 72(4):360–375.  https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20791CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Bouchet H, Koda H, Lemasson A (2017) Age-dependent change in attention paid to vocal exchange rules in Japanese macaques. Anim Behav 129:81–92.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.05.012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Briseno-Jaramillo M, Ramos-Fernández G, Palacios-Romo TM, Sosa-López JR, Lemasson A, Sosa-López JR, Lemasson A (2018) Age and social affinity effects on contact call interactions in free-ranging spider monkeys. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 72:192.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-018-2615-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Calame-Griaule G (1965) Ethnologie et langage. la parole chez les dogon. Gallimard, ParisGoogle Scholar
  15. Candiotti A, Zuberbuhler K, Lemasson A (2012) Convergence and divergence in Diana monkey vocalizations. Biol Lett 8(3):382–385.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2011.1182CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Carter GG, Skowronski MD, Faure PA, Fenton B (2008) Antiphonal calling allows individual discrimination in white-winged vampire bats. Anim Behav 76:1343–1355.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.04.023CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Caskey M, Stephens B, Tucker R, Vohr B (2014) Adult talk in the NICU with preterm infants and developmental outcomes. Pediatrics 133(3):e578–e584.  https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2013-0104CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Chartrand TL, van Baaren R (2009) Human mimicry. Adv Exp Soc Psychol 41:219–274.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)00405-XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Chen HC, Kaplan G, Rogers LJ (2009) Contact calls of common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus): influence of age of caller on antiphonal calling and other vocal responses. Am J Primatol 71:165–170.  https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20636CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Choi JY, Takahashi DY, Ghazanfar AA (2015) Cooperative vocal control in marmoset monkeys via vocal feedback. J Neurophysiol 114:274–283.  https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00228.2015CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  21. Chow CP, Mitchell JF, Miller CT (2015) Vocal turn-taking in a non-human primate is learned during ontogeny. Proc R Soc B Biol Sci 282:20150069.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2015.0069CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Clark AP, Wrangham RW (1993) Acoustic analysis of wild chimpanzee pant hoots: do Kibale Forest chimpanzees have an acoustically distinct food arrival pant hoot? Am J Primatol.  https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.1350310203
  23. Clutton-Brock TH, Albon SD (1979) The roaring of Red Deer and the evolution of honest advertisement. Behaviour 69(4):145–170CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Cossette L, Malcuit G, Pomerleau A, Julien D (1986) Temporal structure of maternal language directed at infants 3 months old. Can J Psychol 40(4):414–422CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Coudé G, Ferrari PF, Rodà F, Maranesi M, Borelli E, Veroni V, Monti F, Fogassi L (2011) Neurons controlling voluntary vocalization in the macaque ventral premotor cortex. PLoS One 6(11):e2682.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0026822CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Cui X, Bryant DM, Reiss AL (2012) NIRS-based hyperscanning reveals increased interpersonal coherence in superior frontal cortex during cooperation. NeuroImage 59(3):2430–2437.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.003CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. De Marco A, Rebout N, Massiot E, Sanna A, Sterck EHM, Langermans JAM, Cozzolino R, Thierry B, Lemasson A (2019) Differential patterns of vocal similarity in tolerant and intolerant macaques. Behaviour 1(aop):1–25.  https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539X-00003562CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Dediu D, Levinson SC (2013) On the antiquity of language: the reinterpretation of Neandertal linguistic capacities and its consequences. Front Psychol 4(397):1–17.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00397CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Digweed SM, Fedigan LM, Rendall D (2007) Who cares who calls? Selective responses to the lost calls of socially dominant group members in the white-faced capuchin (Cebus capucinus). Am J Primatol 69:829–835.  https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20398CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Dunbar RIM (1991) Functional significance of social grooming in primates. Folia Primatol 57:121–131CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Dunbar RIM (1996) Grooming, gossip and the evolution of language. Faber & Faber, LondonGoogle Scholar
  32. Dunbar RIM (2009) The social brain hypothesis and its implications for social evolution. Ann Hum Biol 36(5):562–572.  https://doi.org/10.1080/03014460902960289CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  33. Dunbar RIM, Marriott A, Duncan NDC (1997) Human conversational behavior. Hum Nat 8(3):231–246.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02912493CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Duncan S (1972) Some signals and rules for taking speaking turns in conversations. Soc Psychol 23(2):283–292Google Scholar
  35. Fedurek P, Schel AM, Slocombe KE (2013) The acoustic structure of chimpanzee pant-hooting facilitates chorusing. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 67(11):1781–1789.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-013-1585-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Ferrari PF, Gallese V, Rizzolatti G, Fogassi L (2003) Mirror neurons responding to the observation of ingestive and communicative mouth actions in the monkey ventral premotor cortex. Eur J Neurosci 17(8):1703–1714.  https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02601.xCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Fogassi L, Ferrari PF (2007) Mirror neurons and the evolution of embodied language. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 16(136).  https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2005.0202
  38. Forester DC, Harrison WK (2008) The significance of persistent vocalisation by the spring peeper, Pseudacris Crucifer (Anura: Hylidae). Behaviour 103(1/3):1–15.  https://doi.org/10.1163/156853989x00303CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Fuentes A (1998) Re-evaluating primate monogamy. Am Anthropol 100(4):890–907CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Gallois C, Ogay T, Giles H (2005) Communication accommodation theory a look back and a look ahead. In: Gudykunst B (ed) Theorizing about intercultural communication. Sage, Thousand Oaks, pp 121–148Google Scholar
  41. Geissmann T (2002) Duet-splitting and the evolution of gibbon songs. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc 77:57–76.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S1464793101005826CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Geissmann T, Orgeldinger M (2000) The relationship between duet songs and pair bonds in siamangs, Hylobates syndactylus. Anim Behav 60:805–809.  https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2000.1540CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Ghazanfar AA, Smith-Rohrberg D, Pollen AA, Hauser MD (2002) Temporal cues in the antiphonal long-calling behaviour of cottontop tamarins. Anim Behav 64:427–438.  https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2002.3074CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Ghiglione R (1986) L’homme communiquant. (A. Colin, Ed.). Paris, FranceGoogle Scholar
  45. Ginsburg GP, Kilbourne BK (1988) Emergence of vocal alternation in mother-infant interchanges. J Child Lang 15:221–235.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900012344CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  46. Golinkoff RM (1986) ‘I beg your pardon?’: The preverbal negotiation of failed messages. J Child Lang 21:205–226.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900006826CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Golinkoff RM (1993) When is communication a “meeting of minds”? J Child Lang 20:199–207CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Goll Y, Demartsev V, Koren L, Geffen E (2017) Male hyraxes increase countersinging as strangers become ‘nasty neighbours’. Anim Behav 134:9–14.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.10.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Gregory SW (1990) Analysis of fundamental frequency reveals covariation in interview partners’ speech. J Nonverbal Behav 14(4):237–251.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00989318CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Griffin ZM, Bock K (2000) What the eyes say about speaking. Psychol Sci 11(4):274–279.  https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00255CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Grubb P, Butynski TM, Oates JF, Bearder SK, Disotell TR, Groves CP, Struhsaker TT (2003) Assessment of the diversity of African Primates. Int J Primatol 24(6):1301–1357CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Hage SR, Nieder A (2013) Single neurons in monkey prefrontal cortex encode volitional initiation of vocalizations. Nat Commun 4:2409.  https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms3409CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  53. Haimoff EH (1986) Convergence in the duetting of monogamous Old World primates. J Hum Evol 15(1):51–59.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0047-2484(86)80065-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Hall ET (1959) The silent language. Doubleday, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  55. Hartbauer M, Kratzer S, Steiner K, Römer H (2005) Mechanisms for synchrony and alternation in song interactions of the bushcricket Mecopoda elongata (Tettigoniidae: Orthoptera). J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 191(2):175–188.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-004-0586-4CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Hauser MD, Marler P (1993) Food-associated calls in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): II. Costs and benefits of call production and suppression. Behav Ecol 4:206–212CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Hedwig B (2006) Pulses, patterns and paths: neurobiology of acoustic behaviour in crickets. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-006-0115-8
  58. Heldner M, Edlund J (2010) Pauses, gaps and overlaps in conversations. J Phon 38(4):555–568.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2010.08.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Henry L, Craig AJFK, Lemasson A, Hausberger M (2015) Social coordination in animal vocal interactions. Is there any evidence of turn-taking? The starling as an animal model. Front Psychol 6:1416.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01924CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  60. Hilbrink EE, Gattis M, Levinson SC (2015) Early developmental changes in the timing of turntaking: a longitudinal study of mother-infant interaction. Front Psychol 6:1492.  https://doi.org/10.3389/978-2-88919-825-2CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Hyman J (2003) Countersinging as a signal of aggression in a territorial songbird. Anim Behav 65(6):1179–1185.  https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2003.2175CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Indefrey P, Levelt WJM (2004) The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components. Cognition 92(1–2):101–144.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2002.06.001CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  63. Jaffe J, Beebe B, Feldstein S, Crown CL, Jasnow MD (2001) Rhythms of dialogue in infancy: coordinated timing in development. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 66(2): 1–132. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11428150
  64. Jiang J, Dai B, Peng D, Zhu C, Liu L, Lu C (2012) Neural synchronization during face-to-face communication. J Neurosci 32(45):16064–16069.  https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.2926-12.2012CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  65. Jordan K, Weiss D, Hauser M, Mcmurray B (2004) Antiphonal responses to loud contact calls produced by Saguinus oedipus. Int J Primatol 25(2):465–475CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Jürgens U (1995) Neuronal control of vocal production in non-human and human Primates. In: Zimmermann JU, Newman JD (eds) Current topics in primate vocal communication. Springer, Boston, pp 199–206.  https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-9930-9_10CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Jürgens U, Ploog D (1970) Cerebral representation of vocalization in the squirrel monkey. Exp Brain Res 10(5):532–554.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00234269CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  68. Jürgens U, Maurus M, Ploog D, Winter P (1967) Vocalization in the squirrel monkey (Saimiri sciureus) elicited by brain stimulation. Exp Brain Res 4:114–117.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00240356CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  69. Jürgens U, Lu C-L, Quondamatteo F (1996) C-fos expression during vocal mobbing in the new world monkey Saguinus fuscicollis. Eur J Neurosci 8(1):2–10.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.1996.tb01162.xCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  70. Kappeler PM, Van Schaik CP (2002) Evolution of primate social systems. Int J Primatol 23(4):707–740CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Keller H, Lohaus A, Völker S, Cappenberg M, Chasiotis A (1999) Temporal contingency as an independent component of parenting behavior. Child Dev 70(2):474–485.  https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00034CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Kerbat-Orecchioni C (1990) Les interactions verbales. Armand Collin, ParisGoogle Scholar
  73. Koda H (2004) Flexibility and context-sensitivity during the vocal exchange of coo calls in wild Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata yakui). Behaviour 141(10):1279–1296.  https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539042729685CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Kohler E, Keysers C, Umiltà MA, Fogassi L, Gallese V, Rizzolatti G (2002) Hearing sounds, understanding actions: action representation in Mirror neurons. Science 297:846–848CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Kurtić E, Brown GJ, Wells B (2013) Resources for turn competition in overlapping talk. Speech Comm 55:1–23.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.specom.2012.10.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Laporte MNC, Zuberbühler K (2010) Vocal greeting behaviour in wild chimpanzee females. Anim Behav 80(3):467–473.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2010.06.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Leavens DA, Hostetter AB, Wesley MJ, Hopkins WD (2004) Tactical use of unimodal and bimodal communication by chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes. Anim Behav 67(3):467–476.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.04.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Leavens DA, Russell JL, Hopkins WD (2009) Multimodal communication by captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Anim Cogn 13(1):33–40.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-009-0242-zCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  79. Lemasson A (2011) What can forest guenons « tell » us about the origin of language? In: Vilain A, Schwartz JL, Vauclair JC (eds) Primate communication and human language: vocalisation, gestures, imitation and deixis in humans and non-humans. John Benjamins Publishing, Amsterdam, pp 39–70CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Lemasson A, Hausberger M (2004) Patterns of vocal sharing and social dynamics in a captive group of Campbell’s monkeys (Cercopithecus campbelli campbelli). J Comp Psychol 118(3):347–359.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.118.3.347CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  81. Lemasson A, Hausberger M (2011) Acoustic variability and social significance of calls in female Campbell’s monkeys (Cercopithecus campbelli campbelli). J Acoust Soc Am 129(5):3341–3352.  https://doi.org/10.1121/1.3569704CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  82. Lemasson A, Gautier JP, Hausberger M (2003) Vocal similarities and social bonds Campbell’s monkey (Cercopithecus campbelli). C R Biol 326(12):1185–1193.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.crvi.2003.10.005CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  83. Lemasson A, Hausberger M, Zuberbühler K (2005) Socially meaningful vocal plasticity in adult Campbell’s monkeys (Cercopithecus campbelli). J Comp Psychol 119:220–229.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.119.2.220CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  84. Lemasson A, Blois-Heulin C, Jubin R, Hausberger M (2006) Female social relationships in a captive group of Campbell’s monkeys (Cercopithecus campbelli campbelli). Am J Primatol 68:1161–1170.  https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20315CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  85. Lemasson A, Gandon E, Hausberger M (2010) Attention to elders’ voice in non-human primates. Biol Lett 6(3):325–328.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2009.0875CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. Lemasson A, Glas L, Barbu S, Lacroix A, Guilloux M, Remeuf K, Koda H (2011a) Youngsters do not pay attention to conversational rules: is this so for nonhuman primates? Sci Rep 1:12–15.  https://doi.org/10.1038/srep00022CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Lemasson A, Ouattara K, Petit EJ, Zuberbühler K (2011b) Social learning of vocal structure in a nonhuman primate? BMC Evol Biol 11(362):1–7.  https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2148-11-362CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Lemasson A, Guilloux M, Rizaldi, Barbu S, Lacroix A, Koda H (2013a) Age- and sex-dependent contact call usage in Japanese macaques. Primates 54:283–291.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329-013-0347-5CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  89. Lemasson A, Ouattara K, Zuberbühler K (2013b) Exploring the gaps between primate calls and human language. In: Botha M, Everaert R (eds) The evolutionary emergence of language: evidence and inference. Oxford University Press, Utrecht, pp 181–203CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Lemasson A, Jubin R, Masataka N, Arlet M (2016) Copying hierarchical leaders’ voices? Acoustic plasticity in female Japanese macaques. Sci Rep 6(21289).  https://doi.org/10.1038/srep21289
  91. Lemasson A, Pereira H, Levréro F (2018) Social basis of vocal interactions in Western lowland gorillas (Gorilla g. gorilla). J Comp Psychol 132(2):141–151.  https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000105CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  92. Levinson SC (1983) Pragmatics. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Levinson SC (2016) Turn-taking in human communication – origins and implications for language processing. Trends Cogn Sci 20(1):6–14.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.10.010CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  94. Levinson SC, Holler J (2014) The origin of human multi-modal communication. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 369(1651).  https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0302
  95. Levréro F, Touitou S, Frédet J, Nairaud B, Guéry J-P, Lemasson A (2019) Social bonding drives vocal exchanges in bonobos. Sci Rep 9:711.  https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-36024-9CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  96. Lindsay L, Gambi C, Rabagliati H (2019) Preschoolers optimize the timing of their conversational turns through flexible coordination of language comprehension and production. Psychol Sci 30(4):504–515CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. MacKinnon J, MacKinnon K (1980) The behavior of wild spectral tarsiers. Int J Primatol 1(4):361–379.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02692280CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. MacLarnon A, Hewitt G (2004) Increased breathing control: another factor in the evolution of human language. Evol Anthropol 13:181–197.  https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.20032CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. Masataka N (2003) The onset of language. In: The onset of language. Cambridge University Press.  https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511489754
  100. Masataka N, Biben M (1987) Temporal rules regulating affiliative vocal exchanges of squirrel monkeys. Behaviour 101:311–319CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  101. McComb K, Semple S (2005) Coevolution of vocal communication and sociality in primates. Biol Lett 1(4):381–385.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2005.0366CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  102. McGarva AR, Warner RM (2003) Attraction and social coordination: mutual entrainment of vocal activity rhythms. J Psycholinguist Res 32(3):335–354.  https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023547703110CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  103. Mennill DJ, Boag PT, Ratcliffe LM (2003) The reproductive choices of eavesdropping female black-capped chickadees, Poecile atricapillus. Naturwissenschaften 90(12):577–582.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00114-003-0479-3CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  104. Miller CT, Wang X (2006) Sensory-motor interactions modulate a primate vocal behavior: antiphonal calling in common marmosets. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 192:27–38.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-005-0043-zCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  105. Miller CT, DiMauro A, Pistorio A, Hendry S, Wang X (2010) Vocalization induced CFos expression in marmoset cortex. Front Integr Neurosci 4(128).  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2010.00128
  106. Mitani JC, Nishida T (1993) Contexts and social correlates of long-distance calling by male chimpanzees. Anim Behav 45(4):735–746.  https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1993.1088CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. Nguyen N, Delvaux V (2015) Role of imitation in the emergence of phonological systems. J Phon 53:46–54.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2015.08.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  108. Nishitani N, Schürmann M, Amunts K, Hari R (2005) Broca’s region: from action to language. Physiology 20(1):60–69.  https://doi.org/10.1152/physiol.00043.2004CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  109. Nummela SU, Jovanovic V, de la Mothe L, Miller CT (2017) Social context-dependent activity in marmoset frontal cortex populations during natural conversations. J Neurosci 37(29):7036–7047.  https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0702-17.2017CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  110. Okobi DEJ, Banerjee A, Matheson AMM, Phelps SM, Long MA (2019) Motor cortical control of vocal interaction in neotropical singing mice. Science 363(6430):979–983.  https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aau1758CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  111. Ouattara K, Lemasson A, Zuberbuhler K (2009a) Campbell’s monkeys concatenate vocalizations into context-specific call sequences. Proc Natl Acad Sci 106:22026–22031.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0908118106CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  112. Ouattara K, Lemasson A, Zuberbühler K (2009b) Campbell’s monkeys use affixation to alter call meaning. PLoS One 4:e7808.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0007808CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  113. Papoušek M, Papoušek H (1989) Forms and functions of vocal matching in interactions between mothers and their precanonical infants. First Lang 9(6):137–157.  https://doi.org/10.1177/014272378900900603CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  114. Pardo JS, Gibbons R, Suppes A, Krauss RM (2012) Phonetic convergence in college roommates. J Phon 40:190–197.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2011.10.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  115. Pika S, Wilkinson R, Kendrick KH, Vernes SC (2018) Taking turns: bridging the gap between human and animal communication. Proc R Soc B 285(1880):1–9.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2018.0598CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  116. Pollick AS, Gouzoules H, De Waal FBM (2005) Audience effects on food calls in captive brown capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella. Anim Behav 70(6):1273–1281.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.03.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  117. Pougnault L, LevrÕro F, Mulot B, Lemasson A (2020) Breaking conversational rules matters to captive gorillas: A playback experiment. Sci Rep in pressGoogle Scholar
  118. Reby D, McComb K (2003) Vocal communication and reproduction in deer. Adv Study Behav 33:231–264.  https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-3454(03)33005-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  119. Reissland N, Stephenson T (1999) Turn-taking in early vocal interaction: A comparison of premature and term infants’ vocal interaction with their mothers. Child Care Health Dev 25(6):447–456.  https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2214.1999.00109.xCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  120. Rizzolatti G, Arbib MA (1998) Language within our grasp. Trends Neurosci 21(5):188–194CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  121. Roberts F, Margutti P, Takano S (2011) Judgments concerning the valence of inter-turn silence across speakers of American English, Italian, and Japanese. Discourse Process 48:331–354.  https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2011.558002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  122. Sacks H, Schegloff EA, Jefferson G (1974) A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking for conversation. Language 50:696–735.  https://doi.org/10.2307/412243CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  123. Schaffer R (1977) Study on mother-infant interaction. Academic Press, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  124. Schamberg I, Cheney DL, Clay Z, Hohmann G, Seyfarth RM (2016) Call combinations, vocal exchanges and interparty movement in wild bonobos. Anim Behav 122:109–116.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.10.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  125. Schegloff EA (2002) Overlapping talk and the organization of turn-taking for conversation. Lang Soc 29(01):1–63.  https://doi.org/10.1017/s0047404500001019CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  126. Schwartz JJ (1987) The function of call alternation in anuran amphibians: a test of three hypotheses. Evolution 41(3):461.  https://doi.org/10.2307/2409249CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  127. Sewall KB, Young AM, Wright TF (2016) Social calls provide novel insights into the evolution of vocal learning. Anim Behav 120:163–172.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2016.07.031CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  128. Silk JB (2002) Grunts, Girneys, and good intentions: the origins of strategic commitment in nonhuman primates. In: Nesse R (ed) Commitment: evolutionary perspectives. Russell Sage Press, New York, pp 138–157Google Scholar
  129. Simões CS, Vianney PVR, de Moura MM, Freire MAM, Mello LE, Samesshima K, Araùjo J, Nicolelis M, Ribeiro S (2010) Activation of frontal neocortical areas by vocal production in marmosets. Front Integr Neurosci 4:1–12.  https://doi.org/10.3389/fnint.2010.00123CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  130. Slocombe KE, Kaller T, Turman L, Townsend SW, Papworth S, Squibbs P, Zuberbühler K (2010) Production of food-associated calls in wild male chimpanzees is dependent on the composition of the audience. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 64(12):1959–1966.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s00265-010-1006-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  131. Snowdon CT, Cleveland J (1984) “Conversations” among pygmy marmosets. Am J Primatol 7(1):15–20.  https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.1350070104CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  132. Snowdon CT, Elowson AM (1999) Pygmy marmosets modify call structure when paired. Ethology 105(10):893–908.  https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1439-0310.1999.00483.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  133. Soltis J, Bernhards D, Donkin H, Newman JD (2002) Squirrel monkey chuck call: vocal response to playback chucks based on acoustic structure and affiliative relationship with the caller. Am J Primatol 7:119–130.  https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.10039CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  134. Stephens GJ, Silbert LJ, Hasson U (2010) Speaker-listener neural coupling underlies successful communication. Proc Natl Acad Sci 107(32):14425–14430.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1008662107CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  135. Stern DN, Gibbon J (1979) Temporal expectancies of social behaviours in mother-infant play. In: Thoman EB (ed) Origins of the infant’s social responsiveness. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, pp 409–429Google Scholar
  136. Stern DN, Jaffe J, Beebe B, Bennett SL (1975) Vocalizing in unison and in alternation: two modes of communication within the mother- infant dyad. Ann N Y Acad Sci 263(1):89–100.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1975.tb41574.xCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  137. Stern DN, Beebe B, Jaffe J, Bennett SL (1977) The infant’s stimulus world during social interaction: a study of caregiver behaviors with particular reference to repetition and timing. In: Schaffer HR (ed) Studies in mother–infant interaction. Academic Press, New York, pp 177–202Google Scholar
  138. Stivers T, Enfield NJ, Brown P, Englert C, Hayashi M, Heinemann T, Hoymann G, Rossano F, de Ruiter J, Levinson SC (2009) Universals and cultural variation in turn-taking in conversation. Proc Natl Acad Sci 106(26):10587–10592.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0903616106CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  139. Sugiura H (1993) Temporal and acoustic correlates in vocal exchange of coo calls in Japanese macaques. Behaviour 124:3–4.  https://doi.org/10.1163/156853993X00588CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  140. Sugiura H (1998) Matching of acoustic features during the vocal exchange of coo calls by Japanese macaques. Anim Behav 55:673–687.  https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1997.0602CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  141. Sugiura H (2007) Adjustment of temporal call usage during vocal exchange coo calls in Japanese macaques. Ethology 113:528–533.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2007.01349.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  142. Sugiura H, Masataka N (1995) Temporal and acoustic flexibility in vocal exchanges of coo calls in Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). In: Zimmermann E, Newman JD, Jürgens U (eds) Current topics in primate vocal communication. Plenum Press, New York, pp 121–140.  https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4757-9930-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  143. Sutton D, Larson C, Lindeman RC (1974) Neocortical and limbic lesion effects on primate phonation. Brain Res 71:61–75CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  144. Takahashi DY, Narayanan DZ, Ghazanfar AA (2013) Coupled oscillator dynamics of vocal turn-taking in monkeys. Curr Biol 23:2162–2168.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.09.005CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  145. Takahashi DY, Fenley AR, Ghazanfar AA (2016) Early development of turn-taking with parents shapes vocal acoustics in infant marmoset monkeys. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci 371(1693):20150370.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0370CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  146. Tanaka T, Sugiura H, Masataka N (2006) Cross-sectional and longitudinal studies of the development of group differences in acoustic features of coo calls in two groups of Japanese macaques. Ethology 112(1):7–21.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01103.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  147. Ter Maat M, Truong KP, Heylen D (2010) How turn-taking strategies influence users’ impressions of an agent. In: Intelligent virtual agents: proceedings of the 10th international conference, IVA, pp 441–453.  https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-15892-6_48
  148. Thierry B (2007) Unity in diversity: lessons from macaque societies. Evol Anthropol 16(6):224–238.  https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.20147CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  149. Thierry B, Iwaniuk N, Pellis SM (2000) The influence of phylogeny on the social behaviour of macaques (Primates: Cercopithecidae, genus Macaca). Ethology 106:713–728CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  150. Toarmino CR, Wong L, Miller CT (2017) Audience affects decision-making in a marmoset communication network. Biol Lett 13(1):20160934.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2016.0934CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  151. Trudgill P (2008) Colonial dialect contact in the history of European languages: on the irrelevance of identity to new-dialect formation. Lang Soc 37(2):241–254.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S0047404508080287CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  152. Vehrencamp SL, Ellis JM, Cropp BF, Koltz JM (2014) Negotiation of territorial boundaries in a songbird. Behav Ecol 25(6):1436–1450.  https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/aru135CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  153. Wilson M, Wilson TP (2005) An oscillator model of the timing of turn-taking. Psychon Bull Rev 12(6):957–968.  https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03206432CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  154. Yamaguchi C, Izumi A, Nakamura K (2009) Temporal rules in vocal exchanges of phees and trills in common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). Am J Primatol 71:617–622.  https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20697CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  155. Yoshida S, Okanoya K (2005) Evolution of turntaking: a bio-cognitive perspective. Cogn Stud 12(3):153–165Google Scholar
  156. Zelick R, Narins PM (1985) Characterization of the advertisement call oscillator in the frog Eleutherodactylus coqui. J Comp Physiol A 156(2):223–229.  https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00610865CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd. 2020

Authors and Affiliations

  • Loïc Pougnault
    • 1
    • 2
    • 3
  • Florence Levréro
    • 2
  • Alban Lemasson
    • 1
    Email author
  1. 1.Univ Rennes, Normandie Univ, CNRS, EthoS (Éthologie animale et humaine) – UMR 6552, F-35000RennesFrance
  2. 2.Université de Lyon/Saint-Etienne, CNRS, Equipe Neuro-Ethologie Sensorielle, ENES/CRNL, UMR5292, INSERM UMR_S 1028Saint-EtienneFrance
  3. 3.ZooParc de Beauval & Beauval NatureSaint AignanFrance

Personalised recommendations