Advertisement

Learning Behaviors Among Neanderthals and Palaeolithic Modern Humans: An Introduction

  • Yoshihiro NishiakiEmail author
  • Olaf Jöris
Chapter
Part of the Replacement of Neanderthals by Modern Humans Series book series (RNMH)

Abstract

The learning hypothesis put forth by the Replacement of Neanderthals by Modern Humans (RNMH) Project surmises the primary importance of the innate cognitive difference in learning abilities underlying RNMH processes. An overview of recent studies, supplemented by the contributions in this volume, shows that some of the available evidence is in support of this view, while another portion is not. This is perhaps because learning behaviors in human communities are conditioned by numerous factors, which include not only cognitive abilities in certain realms but also socio-cultural learning environments, as a product of historical developments. While learning undoubtedly played a significant role in the development of adaptive strategies among both Neanderthals and modern humans, a more balanced model seems more appropriate to explain the advantage of modern human lifeways resulting from newer and more effective learning strategies.

Keywords

Learning behavior Cultural evolution Adaptive strategies Cognitive abilities Social environments 

References

  1. Akazawa T, Nishiaki Y, Aoki K (eds) (2013) Dynamics of learning in Neanderthals and modern humans. Vol. 1: cultural perspectives. Springer, TokyoGoogle Scholar
  2. Akazawa T, Ogihara N, Tanabe HC, Terashima H (eds) (2014) Dynamics of learning in Neanderthals and modern humans, vol. 2: cognitive and physical perspectives. Springer, TokyoGoogle Scholar
  3. Akazawa T (2010) New research project entitled “Replacement of Neanderthals by modern humans: testing evolutionary models of learning.” In: Akazawa T (ed) The first Conference on Replacement of Neanderthals by Modern Humans: Testing Evolutionary Models of Learning. Proceedings of the 1st conference on RNMH, Tokyo, pp 29–32Google Scholar
  4. Aoki K (2013) Determinants of cultural evolutionary rates. In: Akazawa T, Nishiaki Y, Aoki K (eds) Dynamics of learning in Neanderthals and modern humans vol.1: cultural perspectives. Springer, Tokyo, pp 199–210CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Aoki K, Mesoudi A (2015) Introduction to “learning strategies and cultural evolution during the Palaeolithic”. In: Aoki K, Mesoudi A (eds) Learning strategies and cultural evolution during the Palaeolithic. Springer, Tokyo, pp 1–8Google Scholar
  6. Bar-Yosef O (2013) Neanderthals and modern humans across Eurasia. In: Akazawa T, Nishiaki Y, Aoki K (eds) Dynamics of learning in Neanderthals and modern humans, vol 1. Springer, Tokyo, pp 7–20CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Beyene Y, Katoh S, WoldeGabriel G, Hart WK, Uto K, Sudo M, Kondo M, Hyodo M, Renne PR, Suwa G, Asfaw B (2013) The characteristics and chronology of the earliest Acheulean at Konso, Ethiopia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 110(5):1584–1591CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Box HO, Gibson KR (1999) Mammalian social learning: comparative and ecological perspectives (no. 72). Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  9. Bruner B, Lozano M (2014) Extended mind and visuo-spatial integration: three hands for the Neandertal lineage. J Archaeol Sci 92:273–280Google Scholar
  10. Bruner E, Ogihara N, Tanabe HC (2018) Digital endocasts: from skulls to brains. Springer Nature, SingaporeCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Cârciumaru M, Niţu EC, Cîrstina O (2015) A geode painted with ochre by the Neanderthal man. Comptes Rendus Palevol 14(1):31–41CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Collard M, Buchanan B, O’Brien MJ (2013) Population size as an explanation for patterns in the Paleolithic archaeological record: more caution is needed. Curr Anthropol 54(S8):S388–S396CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Coolidge FL, Wynn T (2009) The rise of Homo sapiens: the evolution of modern thinking. Wiley-Blackwell, ChichesterCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. David B, Garcia D (2018) Néandertal. Muséum national d'histoire naturelle, ParisGoogle Scholar
  15. Derex M, Boyd R (2016) Partial connectivity increases cultural accumulation within groups. Proc Natl Acad Sci 113(11):2982–2987Google Scholar
  16. Diamond JM (1998) Guns, germs and steel: a short history of everybody for the last 13,000 years. Random House, LondonGoogle Scholar
  17. Douka K, Spinapolice EE (2014) Neanderthal shell tool production: evidence from Middle Paleolithic Italy and Greece. J World Prehistory 25:45–79CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Finlayson C, Brown K, Blasco R, Rosell J, Negro JJ, Bortolotti GR, Finlayson G, Sánchez Marco A, Pacheco FG, Rodriguez Vidal J, Carrion JS, Fa DA, Rodriguez Llanes JM (2012) Birds of a feather: Neanderthal exploitation of raptors and corvids. PLoS One 7(9):e45927.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0045927CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Furuichi T, Sanz C, Koops K, Sakamaki T, Ryu H, Tokuyama N, Morgan D (2015) Why do wild bonobos not use tools like chimpanzees do? Behaviour 152(3–4):425–460CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Gowlett J, Gamble C, Dunbar R (2012) Human evolution and the archaeology of the social brain. Curr Anthropol 53(6):693–722CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Hardy BL, Moncel MH, Daujeard C, Fernandes P, Béarez P, Desclaux E, Gema M, Navarro C, Puaud S, Gallotti R (2013) Impossible Neanderthals? Making string, throwing projectiles and catching small game during Marine Isotope Stage 4 (Abri du Maras, France). Quat Sci Rev 82:23–40CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hawcroft J, Dennell R (2001) Neanderthal cognitive life history and its implications for material culture. In: Derevenski JS (ed) Children and material culture. Routledge, London, pp 89–99Google Scholar
  23. Hayden B (2013) Neanderthal social structure? Oxford J Archaeol 31(1):1–26CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Henrich J (2004) Demography and cultural evolution: how adaptive cultural processes can produce maladaptive losses—the Tasmanian case. Am Antiq 69(2):197–214CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Henry D (2012) The palimpsest problem, hearth pattern analysis, and Middle Paleolithic site structure. Quat Int 247:246–266CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Hoffmann DL, Standish CD, Pike AWG, García-Diez M, Pettitt PB, Angelucci DE, Villaverde V, Zapata J, Milton JA, Alcolea-González J, Cantalejo-Duarte P, Collado H, de Balbín R, Lorblanchet M, Ramos-Muñoz J, Weniger G-C, Zilhão J (2018) Dates for Neanderthal art and symbolic behaviour are reliable. Nat Ecol Evol 2(7):1044CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Hutson JM, García-Moreno A, Noack ES, Turner E, Villaluenga A, Gaudzinski-Windheuser S (2018) The origins of bone tool technologies. “Retouching the Palaeolithic: becoming human and the origins of bone tool technology” Conference at Schloss Herrenhausen in Hannover, Germany, 21–23. October 2015. Verlag des Römisch-Germanischen Zentralmuseums, MainzGoogle Scholar
  28. Jaubert J, Verheyden S, Genty D, Soulier M, Cheng H, Blamart D, Burlet C, Camus H, Delaby S, Deldicque D, Edwards RL, Ferrier C, Lacrampe-Cuyaubère F, Lévêque F, Maksud F, Mora P, Muth X, Régnier É, Rouzaud J-N, Santos F (2016) Early Neanderthal constructions deep in Bruniquel Cave in southwestern France. Nature 534(7605):111–114CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Kadowaki S (2013) Issues of chronological and geographical distributions of Middle and Upper Palaeolithic cultural variability in the Levant and implications for the learning behavior of Neanderthals and Homo sapiens. In: Akazawa T, Nishiaki Y, Aoki K (eds) Dynamics of learning in Neanderthals and modern humans vol. 1: cultural perspectives. Springer, New York, pp 59–91CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Klein R, Edgar B (2002) The Dawn of human culture. Wiley, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  31. Kochiyama T, Ogihara N, Tanabe HC, Kondo O, Amano H, Hasegawa K, Suzuki H, Ponce de León MS, Zollikofer CPE, Bastir M, Stringer C, Sadato N, Akazawa T (2018) Reconstructing the Neanderthal brain using computational anatomy. Sci Rep 8:6296CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Koops K, Furuichi T, Hashimoto C (2015) Chimpanzees and bonobos differ in intrinsic motivation for tool use. Sci Rep 5:11356CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Kuhn SL (2013) Cultural transmission, institutional continuity and the persistence of the Mousterian. In: Akazawa T, Nishiaki Y, Aoki K (eds) Dynamics of learning in Neanderthals and modern humans vol. 1: cultural perspectives. Springer, New York, pp 105–114CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Lewin R (1998) The origin of modern humans. W.H. Freeman & Company, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  35. Majkić A, d’Errico F, Milošević S, Mihailović D, Dimitrijević V (2018) Sequential incisions on a cave bear bone from the Middle Paleolithic of Pešturina Cave, Serbia. J Archaeol Method Theory 25(1):69–116CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. McBrearty A, Brooks A (2000) The revolution that wasn’t: a new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior. J Hum Evol 39:453–563CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Mesoudi A, Aoki K (eds) (2015) Learning strategies and cultural evolution during the Paleolithic. Springer, New YorkGoogle Scholar
  38. Mithen SJ (1996) The prehistory of the mind: a search for the origins of art, science and religion. Thames & Hudson, LondonGoogle Scholar
  39. Mithen SJ (2005) The singing Neanderthals: the origins of neural development, language, mind and body. Weidenfeld & Nicolson, LondonGoogle Scholar
  40. Mithen SJ (2014) The cognition of Homo neanderthalensis and H. sapiens: does the use of pigment necessarily imply symbolic thought? In: Akazawa T, Ogihara N, Tanabe HC, Terashima (eds) Dynamics of learning in Neanderthals and modern humans vol. 2: cognitive and physical perspectives. Springer, New York, pp 7–16CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Nishiaki Y, Akazawa T (eds) (2018) The middle and upper Paleolithic archeology of the Levant and beyond. Springer Nature, SingaporeGoogle Scholar
  42. Nishiaki Y, Aoki K, Akazawa T (2013) Introduction. In: Akazawa T, Nishiaki Y, Aoki K (eds) Dynamics of learning in Neanderthals and modern humans vol. 1: cultural perspectives. Springer, New York, pp 1–3Google Scholar
  43. Pearce E, Stringer C, Dunbar RIM (2014) New insights into differences in brain organization between Neanderthals and anatomically modern humans. Proc R Soc B 280:20130168.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2013.0168CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Rodríguez-Vidala J, d’Errico F, Pacheco FG, Blasco R, Rosell J, Jennings RP, Queffelec A, Finlayson G, Fa DA, María Gutiérrez Lópezi J, Carrión JS, Negro JJ, Finlayson S, Cáceres LM, Bernal MA, Fernández Jiménez S, Finlayson C (2014) A rock engraving made by Neanderthals in Gibraltar. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 111(37):13301–13306.  https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1411529111CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Shennan S, Cowgill GL, Gosden C, Lyman RL, O’Brien MJ, Neves EG, Pétrequin P, Scarre C, Wotzka H-P (2000) Population, culture history, and the dynamics of culture change. Curr Anthropol 41(5):811–835CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Slimak L, Inge Svendsen J, Mangerud J, Plisson H, Heggen HP, Brugère A, Pavlov PY (2011) Late Mousterian persistence near the Arctic circle. Science 332:841–845CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Soressi M, McPherron SP, Lenoir M, Dogandzic T, Goldberg P, Jacobs Z, Maigrot Y, Martisius NL, Miller CE, Rendu W, Richards R, Skinner MM, Steele TE, Talamo S, Texier J-P (2013) Neandertals made the first specialized bone tools in Europe. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 110:14186–14190CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Spikins P, Hitchens G, Needham A, Rutherford H (2014) The cradle of thought: growth, learning, play and attachment in Neanderthal children. Oxford J Archaeol 33(2):111–134CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Terashima H, Hewlett BS (eds) (2017) Social learning and innovation in contemporary hunter-gatherers. Springer Nature, SingaporeGoogle Scholar
  50. Toth N, Schick KD, Savage-Rumbaugh ES, Sevcik RA, Rumbaugh DM (1993) Pan the tool-maker: investigations into the stone tool-making and tool-using capabilities of a bonobo (Pan paniscus). J Archaeol Sci 20(1):81–91CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Villa P, Roebroeks W (2014) Neandertal demise: an archaeological analysis of the modern human superiority complex. PLoS One 9(4):e96424.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0096424CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Wynn T, Coolidge FL (2011) The implications of the working memory model for the evolution of modern cognition. Int J Evol Biol 2011:741357.  https://doi.org/10.4061/2011/741357CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Yamauchi T (2016) Body growth and life history of modern humans and Neanderthals from the perspective of human evolution. In: Terashima T, Hewlett BS (eds) Social learning and innovation in contemporary hunter-gatherers. Springer Nature, Singapore, pp 285–291CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Zilhão J (2013) Neandertal-modern human contact in Western Eurasia: issues of dating, taxonomy, and cultural associations. In: Akazawa T, Nishiaki Y, Aoki K (eds) Dynamics of learning in Neanderthals and modern humans vol. 1: cultural perspectives. Springer, New York, pp 21–58CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Zilhão J, Angelucci D, Badal-García E, d’Errico F, Daniel F, Dayet L, Douka K, Higham TFG, Martínez-Sánchez MJ, Montes-Bernárdez R, Murcia-Mascarós S, Pérez-Sirvent C, Roldán-García C, Vanhaeren M, Villaverde V, Wood R, Zapata J (2010) Symbolic use of marine shells and mineral pigments by Iberian Neandertals. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107:1023–1028CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd. 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.The University MuseumThe University of TokyoTokyoJapan
  2. 2.MONREPOS Archaeological Research Center and Museum for Human Behavioural Evolution, Schloss MonreposNeuwiedGermany

Personalised recommendations