Advertisement

Adaptation to Provocative Motion

  • Thomas G. DobieEmail author
Chapter
Part of the Springer Series on Naval Architecture, Marine Engineering, Shipbuilding and Shipping book series (NAMESS, volume 6)

Abstract

The question of adaptation is a key issue in dealing with motion sickness. It is generally accepted that most people should be capable of adapting to provocative motion. In that case, why do so many people suffer from chronic motion sickness? As I have said in the previous chapter, I believe that this is due to a psychological component based upon unpleasant motion responses in the past; perhaps as a result of individual motion experiences from a young age. Early and continued exposure to provocative motion may either sensitise a person or allow that person to adapt. This will depend upon the duration, character and frequency of exposure to whatever form of motion. In a sense it may be entirely fortuitous and depend on social and/or geographical circumstances. I shall return to this question later in Chap.  12, when discussing cognitive-behavioural training.

Reference

  1. Benson AJ (1988) Motion sickness. In: Ernsting J, King P (eds) Aviation medicine, 2nd ed. Butterworth-Heinemann Ltd., OxfordGoogle Scholar
  2. Brown JJ, Baloh RW (1987) Persistent mal de debarquement syndrome: a motion-induced subjective disorder of balance. Am J Otolaryngol 8:219–222CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Dobie TG (1974) Airsickness in aircrew. AGARDOGRAPH No. 177, North Atlantic Treaty Organization Advisory Group for Aerospace Research and Development, Neuilly-sur Seine, FranceGoogle Scholar
  4. Dobie TG, May JG (1990) The generalization of tolerance to motion environments. Aviat Space Environ Med 61:707–711Google Scholar
  5. Dobie TG, May JG, Fisher WD, Bologna NB (1989a) An evaluation of cognitive-behavioral therapy for training resistance to visually-induced motion sickness. Aviat Space Environ Med 60:307–314Google Scholar
  6. Dobie TG, May JG, Dunlap WP, Anderson ME (1989b) Reduction of visually-induced motion sickness elicited by changes in illumination wavelength. Aviat Space Environ Med 60:749–754Google Scholar
  7. Dobie TG, May JG, Guttierrez C, Heller S (1990) The transfer of adaptation between actual and simulated rotary stimulation. Aviat Space Environ Med 60:1085–1091Google Scholar
  8. Gibson WC, Manning GW, Kirkpatrick B (1943) Associate Committee on Aviation Medicine. National Research Council, Canada. Report No. C2512. June 1943Google Scholar
  9. Gordon CR, Spitzer O, Shupak A, Doweck H (1992) Survey of mal de debarquement. BMJ 304:544CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Gordon CR, Spitzer O, Doweck I, Melamed Y, Shupak A (1995) Clinical features of mal de debarquement: adaptation and habituation to sea conditions. J Vestib Res 5(5):363–369CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Graybiel A, Kennedy RS, Knoblock EC, Guedry FE, Hertz W, McCleod M, Colehour JK, Miller EF, Fregly A (1965) Effects of exposure to a rotating environment (10 rpm) on four aviators for a period of twelve days. Aerosp Med 36:733–754Google Scholar
  12. Guedry FE (1965a) Orientation of the rotation-axis relative to gravity: its influence on nystagmus and the sensation of rotation. Acta Otolaryngol 60:30–45CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Guedry FE Jr (1965b) Habituation to complex vestibular stimulation in man: transfer and retention of effects from twelve days of rotation at 10 rpm. Percept Mot Skills 21:459–481CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Hain TC, Hanna PA, Rheinberger MA (1999) Mal de debarquement. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 125:615–620CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Hemingway A (1946) Selection of men for aeronautical training based on susceptibility to motion sickness. J Aviat Med 17:153Google Scholar
  16. Homick JL (1979) Space motion sickness. Acta Astronautica 1259–1272CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Howlett JG (1957) Motion sickness. Can Med Assoc J 76:871–873Google Scholar
  18. International Standard ISO 2631-1:1997(E) (7/15/1997) Mechanical vibration and shock—evaluation of human exposure—part 1: general requirements. Irwin JA (1881) The pathology of sea-sickness. Lancet ii:907–909Google Scholar
  19. Joekes AM (1942) Correlation of swing sickness with airsickness. British Flying Personnel Research Committee. Report No. 475Google Scholar
  20. Kennedy RS, Berbaum KS, Williams MC, Brannan J, Welch RB (1987) Transfer of perceptual-motor training and the space adaptation syndrome. Aviat Space Environ Med 58(9, suppl):A29–A33Google Scholar
  21. Lynn R (1966) Attention, arousal and the orientation reaction. Pergamon Press, OxfordGoogle Scholar
  22. Manning GW (1943) Acclimatisation to swing sickness. Associate Committee on Aviation Medical Research. National Research Council, Canada. Report No. C2623, Oct 1943Google Scholar
  23. McBurney D, Collings V (1977) Introduction to sensation/perception. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJGoogle Scholar
  24. Money KE (1970) Motion sickness. Physiol Rev 50:1–38CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Parker DE, Reschke MF, von Gierke HE, Lessard CS (1987) Effects of proposed preflight adaptation training on eye movements, selfmotion perception, and motion sickness: a progress report. Aviat Space Environ Med 58(9, suppl):A42–A49Google Scholar
  26. Pavlov IP (1927) Conditioned reflexes: an investigation of the physiological activity of the cerebral cortex. In: Andreo GV (trans, ed). Oxford University Press, LondonGoogle Scholar
  27. Reason JT, Brand JJ (1975) Motion sickness. Academic Press, New York, NYGoogle Scholar
  28. Reason JT, Graybiel A (1969a) Adaptation to Coriolis accelerations: its transfer to the opposite direction of rotation as a function of intervening activity at zero velocity. NAMI-1086, NASA Order R-93, Naval Aerospace Medical Institute, Pensacola, FLGoogle Scholar
  29. Reason JT, Graybiel A (1969b) An attempt to measure the degree of adaptation produced by differing amounts of Coriolis vestibular stimulation in a slow rotation room. NAMI-1084, NASA Order R-93, Naval Aerospace Medical Institute, Pensacola, FLGoogle Scholar
  30. Reason JT, Graybiel A (1970) Progressive adaptation to Coriolis accelerations associated with 1-rpm increments in the velocity of the slow rotating room. Aerosp Med 41(1):73–79Google Scholar
  31. Tyler DB, Bard P (1949) Motion sickness. Physiol Rev 311–369CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Welch RB (1978) Perceptual modification: adapting to altered sensory environments. Academic Press, New York, NYGoogle Scholar
  33. Wiker SF, Kennedy RS, McCauley ME, Pepper RL (1979) Reliability, validity and application of an improved scale for assessment of motion sickness severity. USCG Technical Report No. CG-D 29-79, U.S. Coast Guard Office of Research and Development, Washington, DCGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.National Biodynamics Laboratory, College of EngineeringUniversity of New OrleansNew OrleansUSA

Personalised recommendations