Advertisement

The Effects of the Type of Rest Breaks on Return-to-Task Performance in Semi-automated Tasks with Varying Complexities

  • S. Zschernack
  • M. Göbel
  • Z. Hoyi
Conference paper
Part of the Advances in Intelligent Systems and Computing book series (AISC, volume 823)

Abstract

Automation in the aviation industry is acknowledged as a useful tool in reducing pilot workload. Different types of rest tasks are commonly prescribed fatigue countermeasures in the industrial setting and have been showed to elicit beneficial effects on prolonged human performance. Understanding the effects of different rest break activity and time out-of-the-loop during semi-automated flying on return to task performance has been adequately studied, thus highlighting its importance in the context of flight safety.

The present study requested participants to perform a tracking task in a laboratory where they changed from activity (30 min) to a break (2 vs. 30 min) and back to the activity (20 min). The task varied in the complexity of the activity (pure tracking vs. tracking plus memory plus rule-based decision making), the type of break (passive rest vs. actively supervising) and the duration of the break (2 min vs. 30 min). Performance was measured as effective response time in the tracking task and number of correct responses to secondary cognitive tasks.

Physiological measures included heart rate (HR), heart rate variability (HRV- time and frequency-domain), eye blink frequency and duration. The Karolinska Sleepiness Scale was used as a subjective measure.

The study concluded that active, administrative tasks, which allowed the operator to maintain some form of situational awareness by monitoring the automated system, achieved favourable effects of being more alert than the passive rest break of being disengaged from the system. The shorter duration of being out-of-the-loop from controlling the system proved to be more advantageous than the longer out-of-the-loop duration. In looking at the workload levels of arousal, the results suggest that the higher workload level is better at maintaining the alertness of operators.

Keywords

Return-to task performance Rest break Semi-automated tasks 

References

  1. 1.
    Alosco ML, Spitznagel MB, Fischer KH, Miller LA, Pillai V, Hughes J, Gunstad J (2012) Both texting and eating are associated with impaired simulated driving performance. Traffic Inj Prev 13(5):468–475Google Scholar
  2. 2.
    Beissmann T (2012) Eating, drinking while driving slows reaction times. http://www.caradvice.com.au/171581/eating-drinking-while-driving-slows-reaction-times-study. Accessed 4 Apr 2016
  3. 3.
    Campbell BN, Smith JD, Najm WG (2002) Examination of Crash Contributing Factors Using National Crash Databases. (Report No. DOT-VNTSC-NHTSA-02-07). Volpe national Transportation Systems Center: Cambridge, MAGoogle Scholar
  4. 4.
    Irwin C, Monement S, Desbrow B (2015) The influence of drinking, texting, and eating on simulated driving performance. Traffic Inj Prev 16(2):116–123CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. 5.
    Jakle JA, Sculle KA (1999) Fast food: roadside restaurants in the automobile age. The John Hopkins University Press, United States of AmericaGoogle Scholar
  6. 6.
    Locher J, Moritz O (2009) Eating while driving causes 80% of all car accidents, study show. http://www.nydailynews.com/new-york/eating-driving-80-car-accidents-study-shows-article-1.427796. Accessed 4 Mar 2016
  7. 7.
    Louw T (2013) An investigation into control mechanisms of driving performance resource depletion and effort regulation. Unpublished Master of Science thesis, Rhodes University, Grahamstown, South AfricaGoogle Scholar
  8. 8.
    Lytx (2014) Lytx Data Finds Three Dangerous Activities You May Be Doing While Driving Every Day. http://www.lytx.com/press-releases/lytx-data-finds-three-dangerous-activities-you-may-be-doing-while-driving-every-day. Accessed 4 Apr 2016
  9. 9.
    Marsh K, Bugusu B (2007) Food packaging—roles, materials, and environmental issues. J Food Sci 72(3):39–55CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. 10.
    Siu J (2013) Eating while driving deadlier than texting behind the wheel, study shows. http://www.autoguide.com/auto-news/2012/05/eating-while-driving-deadlier-than-texting-behind-thewheel-study-shows.html. Accessed 4 Mar 2016
  11. 11.
    Stutts JC, Reinfurt DW, Staplin L, Rodgman EA (2001) The role of driver distraction in traffic crashes. AAA Foundation for Traffic Safety, Washington, DCCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. 12.
    Stutts J, Feaganes J, Reinfurt D, Rodgman E, Hamlett C, Gish K, Staplin L (2005) Driver’s exposure to distractions in their natural driving environment. Accid Anal Prev 37(6):1093–1101CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. 13.
    Tlhoaele K (2013) Understanding the multitask process of eating while driving and its potential effect on driving performance. Unpublished Honours thesis Rhodes University, Grahamstown, South AfricaGoogle Scholar
  14. 14.
    Young K, Lee JD, Regan MA (2008) Defining driver distraction. In: Young K, Lee JD, Regan MA (eds) Driver distraction: theory, effects, and mitigation. CRC Press, Florida, USA, pp. 31–40Google Scholar
  15. 15.
    Young MS, Mahfoud JM, Walker GH, Jenkins DP, Stanton NA (2008) Crash dieting: the effects of eating and drinking on driving performance. Accid Anal Prev 40(1):142–148CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. 16.
    White MP, Eiser JR, Harris PR (2004) Risk perceptions of mobile phone use while driving. Risk Anal 24(2):323–334CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Rhodes UniversityGrahamstownSouth Africa

Personalised recommendations