Advertisement

Investigate the Effect of Age and Lights on Human Responses

  • Chinmei Chou
  • Ruyu Huang
Conference paper
Part of the Advances in Intelligent Systems and Computing book series (AISC, volume 824)

Abstract

The purpose of the study was to investigate the effect of age and lights on human responses. The participants included 10 young (22.40 ± 1.43 years old) and 10 middle-aged (36.40 ± 1.43 years old) adults. Independent variables of the experiment were groups (young and middle-aged adults), light environment (light A and light B) and time. Light A and light B were RGB LEDs; and the peak of blue spectrum was the only difference between light A (464 nm) and light B (451 nm). Dependent variables were physiological responses and subjective evaluations. The physiological responses included heart rate (HR), galvanic skin response (GSR) and little finger temperature. The subjective evaluations included feeling about lights (range from dim to bright), emotion and generalized anxiety disorder questionnaire (GAD). In this experiment, each participant should take a rest and feel the light environment for 40 min. The results showed that the age groups were the main factor to affect the physiological responses. The post-hoc comparisons showed that young adults were more relaxing than middle-aged adults. On interaction between age groups and lighs for GSR and HR, the reults showed that young adults felt more relaxed than middle-aged adults while at light A environment which was 464 nm peak wavelength of blue spectrum. Lights were the main factor to affect the subjective evaluations. All participants felt more dim, less tense, more serene and less GAD at light B than at light A. However, there was significant difference for only tense & jittery question and the others were significant trend (bright/dim, serene & contented and GAD). This study suggested that it was needed to consider the age and light factors when designing the light environment.

Keywords

3000 K light Physiological response Age Blue wavelength Ergonomics 

Notes

Acknowledgments

We would like to acknowledge the lighting equipment support from the ADATA Technology Co., Ltd.

References

  1. Boudewyns PA (1976) A comparison of the effects of stress vs. relaxation instruction on the finger temperature response. Behav Ther 7(1):54–67CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Brainard GC, Hanifin JP, Greeson JM, Byrne B, Glickman G, Gerner E, Rollag MD (2001) Action spectrum for melatonin regulation in humans: evidence for a novel circadian photoreceptor. J Neurosci 21(16):6405–6412CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Cacioppo JT, Tassinary LG, Berntson G (eds) (2007) Handbook of psychophysiology. Cambridge University Press, CambridgeGoogle Scholar
  4. Hyon G, Tai J (2014) The effects of illuminance and correlated color temperature on visual comfort of occupants’ behavior. Korea Inst Ecol Archit Environ J 14(1):23–30Google Scholar
  5. Kraneburg A, Franke S, Methling R, Griefahn B (2017) Effect of color temperature on melatonin production for illumination of working environments. Appl Ergon 58:446–453CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Lang PJ, Greenwald MK, Bradley MM, Hamm AO (1993) Looking at pictures: affective, facial, visceral, and behavioral reactions. Psychophysiology 30(3):261–273CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Morita T, Tokura H (2001) Effects of lights of different color temperature on the nocturnal changes in core temperature and melatonin in humans. Appl Hum Sci 15(5):243–246CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Noguchi H, Sakaguchi T (1999) Effect of illuminance and color temperature on lowering of physiological activity. Appl Hum Sci 18(4):117–123CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Plitnick B, Figueiro MG, Wood B, Rea MS (2010) The effects of red and blue light on alertness and mood at night. Light Res Technol 42:449–458CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Revell VL, Arendt J, Fogg LF, Skene DJ (2006) Alerting effects of light are sensitive to very short wavelengths. Neurosci Lett 399(1):96–100CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Riva G, Davide F, IJsselsteijn WA (2003) 7 measuring presence: subjective, behavioral and physiological methods. In: Being there: concepts, effects and measurement of user presence in synthetic environments, pp 110–118Google Scholar
  12. Russell JA, Barrett LF (1999) Core affect, prototypical emotional episodes, and other things called emotion: dissecting the elephant. J Pers Soc Psychol 76(5):805CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Russell JA, Weiss A, Mendelsohn GA (1989) Affect grid: a single-item scale of pleasure and arousal. J Pers Soc Psychol 57(3):493–502CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Spitzer RL, Kroenke K, Williams JB, Löwe B (2006) A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: the GAD-7. Arch Intern Med 166(10):1092–1097CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Thapan K, Arendt J, Skene DJ (2001) An action spectrum for melatonin suppression: evidence for a novel non-rod, non-cone photoreceptor system in humans. J Physiol 535(1):261–267CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Turner PL, Van Someren EJ, Mainster MA (2010) The role of environmental light in sleep and health: effects of ocular aging and cataract surgery. Sleep Med Rev 14(4):269–280CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Wada K, Yata S, Akimitsu O, Krejci M, Noji T, Nakade M, Takeuchi H, Harada T (2013) A tryptophan-rich breakfast and exposure to light with low color temperature at night improve sleep and salivary melatonin level in Japanese students. J Circadian Rhythm 11(1):4CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Industrial Engineering and ManagementYuan Ze UniversityChung-LiTaiwan, R.O.C.

Personalised recommendations