Advertisement

Consciousness, Value, and Human Nature

  • Grant Gillett
Chapter

Abstract

Human consciousness emerges from neural evolution as a complex and densely woven whole in which doing and perceiving things and the use of language combine to open up the world for human engagement. That integrated whole can be disrupted by breakdowns in its neurological fabric or in its psychological weaving together through human communication and the shared use of symbols and our relationships with each other. When it functions well, that woven whole engenders certain values—the value of crafted things whose purposefulness can be discerned and contemplated but not fully subsumed by functional considerations and the value of our jointly constructed ethos of mutuality, cooperation, and achievement. Those values respectively inform and emerge from the discourses of art and morality.

Keywords

Second nature Value and valuing Rationality sociality and dependency 

References and Bibliography

  1. Apperly, I., Samson, D. E., & Humphreys, G. W. (2005). Domain specificity and theory of mind: Evaluating neuropsychological evidence. Trends in Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(12), 572–577.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  3. Blair, R. J. R. (2007). The amygdala and ventromedial frontal cortex in morality and psychopathy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(9), 387–392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Chemero, A. (2003). An outline of a theory of affordances. Ecological Psychology, 15(2), 181–185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Damasio, A. (2010). Self comes to mind. New York: Random House.Google Scholar
  6. Dapretto, M., Davies, M., et al. (2006). Understanding emotions in others: Mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 9(1), 28–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Davidson, D. (2001). Subjective, intersubjective, objective. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Dunbar, R., & Schulz, S. (2007). Evolution in the social brain. Science, 317, 1344–1347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Ellis, H. D., & Lewis, M. B. (2001). Capgras delusion: A window on face recognition. Trends in Cognitive Neuroscience, 5(4), 149–156.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Foucault, M. (1984). The Foucault reader (P. Rabinow, Ed.). London: Penguin.Google Scholar
  11. Franz, E. A. (2010). A framework for conceptual binding of bimanual actions: Possible applications to neurology and neuro-rehabilitative therapies. Current Trends in Neurology, 4, 1–22.Google Scholar
  12. Fullinwinder, S. (1983). Sigmund Freud, Hughlings Jackson and speech. Journal of the History of Ideas, 44(1), 151–158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Gillett, G. (2004). Bioethics and the clinic: Hippocratic reflections. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
  14. Gillett, G. (2008). Subjectivity and being somebody: Human identity and neuroethics (St Andrews Series on Philosophy and Public Affairs). Exeter: Imprint Academic.Google Scholar
  15. Gillett, G., & Amos C. (2015). The discourse of clinical ethics and the maladies of the soul. In Oxford handbook of psychiatric ethics (Vol. I, Chap. 30). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  16. Gillett, G., & Franz, L. (2014). Evolutionary neurology, responsive equilibrium, and the moral brain. Consciousness and Cognition. Retrieved from http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S105381001400172X
  17. Greene, J. (2003). From neural ‘is’ to moral ‘ought’: What are the moral implications of neuroscientific moral psychology? Nature Neuroscience, 4, 847–850.Google Scholar
  18. Greene, J., & Haidt, J. (2002). How (and where) does moral judgment work? Trends in Cognitive Neuroscience, 6(12), 517–523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Griffiths, P. (1997). What emotions really are. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108(4), 814–834.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Harris, J. C. (2003). Social neuroscience, empathy, brain integration and neurodevelopmental disorders. Physiology and Behavior, 79, 525–531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Hughlings Jackson, J. (1878). On affectations of speech from disease of the brain (1). Brain, I(III), 304–330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Hughlings Jackson, J. (1879). On affectations of speech from disease of the brain (2). Brain, I(III), 203–222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Hughlings Jackson, J. (1884). Croonian lectures on the evolution and dissolution of the nervous system. Lancet: (a) March 29, pp. 555–558; (b) April 12, pp. 649–652; and (c) 26, pp. 739–744.Google Scholar
  25. Hughlings Jackson, J. (1887). Remarks on the evolution and dissolution of the nervous system. British Journal of Psychiatry, 33, 25–48.Google Scholar
  26. Hume, D. (1740 [1969]). A treatise of human nature (E. Mossner, Ed.). London: Penguin.Google Scholar
  27. Hurley, S. (2008). The shared circuits model (SCM): How control, mirroring, and simulation can enable imitation, deliberation and mindreading. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 31, 1–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Ishai, A. (2008). Let’s face it: It’s a cortical network. Neuroimage, 40, 415–419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Kanwisher, N., McDermott, J., & Chun, M. M. (1997). The fusiform face area: A module in Human extrastriate cortex specialized for face perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 17(11), 4302–4311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Lacan, J. (1977). Ecrits (A. Sheridan, Trans.). New York: Norton & Co.Google Scholar
  31. Lamm, C., Batson, C. D., & Decety, J. (2007). The neural substrate of human empathy: Effects of perspective taking and cognitive appraisal. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(1), 42–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Levinas, E. (1996). Basic philosophical writings (A. Peperzak, S. Critchley, & R. Bernasconi, Eds.). Bloomington: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
  33. Lovibond, S. (1983). Realism and imagination in ethics. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  34. Lovibond, S. (2002). Ethical formation. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Luria, A. R. (1973). The working brain. Harmondsworth: Penguin.Google Scholar
  36. McDowell, J. (1994). Mind and world. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  37. McGilchrist, I. (2010). The master and his emissary. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
  38. Mitchell, J. P. (2009). Social psychology as a natural kind. Trends in Cognitive Science, 13(5), 246–251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Motzkin, J. C., Newman, J. P., Kiehl, K. A., & Koenigs, M. (2011). Reduced pre-frontal connectivity in psychopathy. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(4), 17348–17357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Northoff, G., & Bermpohl, F. (2004). Cortical midline structures and the self. Trends in Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 102–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Nussbaum, M. (1990). Love’s knowledge. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  42. Pinker, S. (2010). The cognitive niche: Coevolution of intelligence, sociality and language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(s2), 8993–8999.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Pinker, S. (2011). The better angels of our nature. New York: Allen Lane.Google Scholar
  44. Rawls, J. (1957). Outline of a decision procedure for ethics. Philosophical Review, 66, 177–197.Google Scholar
  45. Saxe, R. (2006). Uniquely human social cognition. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 16, 235–239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Saxe, R., & Wexler, A. (2005). Making sense of another mind: The role of the right temporo-parietal junction. Neuropsychologia, 43, 1391–1399.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Singer, T. (2006). The neuronal basis and ontogeny of empathy and mind reading: Review of literature and implications for future research. Neuroscience & Biobehavioural Reviews, 30, 855–862.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Strawson, P. (1974). Freedom and resentment and other essays. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
  49. Tomasello, M. (2014). The natural history of human thinking. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Williams, B. (1985). Ethics and the limits of philosophy. London: Fontana.Google Scholar
  51. Young, L., Dodell-Feder, D., & Saxe, R. (2010). Who gets the attention of the temporo-parietal junction? Am fMRI investigation of attention and theory of mind. Neuropsychologia, 48, 2658–2664.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© The Author(s) 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Grant Gillett
    • 1
  1. 1.Dunedin School of MedicineUniversity of OtagoDunedinNew Zealand

Personalised recommendations