Advertisement

Now You See It, Now You Don’t: A Change Blindness Assessment of Flight Display Complexity and Pilot Performance

  • Claire McDermott Ealding
  • Alex Stedmon
Conference paper
Part of the Lecture Notes in Computer Science book series (LNCS, volume 10906)

Abstract

Synthetic Vision Systems (SVS) provide a revolutionary new technology for modern aircraft flight decks, changing the way pilots see the world by merging a high-resolution representation of their immediate terrain and surroundings underneath the traditional primary flight instruments. Despite its operational benefits, there may be challenges to the effective use of SVS and little research has focused on pilot performance measures. Using custom designed flight display images and a novel Flicker Paradigm, an experiment was designed to measure pilot response time to visual cues on both SVS and conventional electronic displays and also for different levels of pilot experience. Results indicated that change detection was impaired with the SVS display across the pilot ranks. Pilots were typically seven seconds slower and made more errors using the SVS display, supporting other research that suggests that the background complexity of SVS hampers the speed and accuracy of identifying visual cues. Contrary to what was expected, first officers performed both quicker and more accurately than captains. Perhaps this signals the first signs of a new crop of pilots who have been trained using 21st century synthetic and electronic flight displays in today’s light training aircraft.

Keywords

Avionics Synthetic vision systems Change blindness Pilot performance Flicker paradigm 

Notes

Funding

This research was part of an MSc Thesis and therefore not formally funded. The authors acknowledge the support of Zetta Jet Flight Department, specifically Eric Rastler Chief Pilot, for granting approval to conduct the research using company personnel.

References

  1. 1.
    Endsley, M.: Towards a theory of situation awareness in dynamic systems. Hum. Factors 37(1), 32–64 (1995)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. 2.
    Endsley, M., Jones, D.: Designing for Situation Awareness: An Approach to User Centred Design, 2nd edn. CRC Press, Boca Raton (2016)Google Scholar
  3. 3.
    Foyle, D.C., Kaiser, M.K., Johnson, W.W.: Visual cues in low-level flight: implications for pilotage, training, simulation, and enhanced/synthetic vision systems. In: American Helicopter Society 48th Annual Forum, vol. 1, pp. 253–260 (1992)Google Scholar
  4. 4.
    Wilmhelmson, R.B., Jewett, B.F., Shaw, C., Wicker, L.J., Arrott, M., Bushell, C.B., Bajuk, M., Thingvold, J., Yost, J.B.: A study of the evolution of a numerically modelled severe storm. Int. J. Supercomput. Appl. 4(2), 20–36 (1990)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. 5.
    Medjal, S., McCauley, E., Beringer, D.: Human Factors Design Guidelines for Multifunction Displays. U.S. Department of Transportation Office of Aerospace Medicine: Washington (2001)
Google Scholar
  6. 6.
    Priznell III, L., Kramer, L., Bailey, R., Arthur, J., Williams, S., McNabb, J.: Augmentation of Cognition and Perception Through Advanced Synthetic Vision Technology. NASA Langley, Hampton (2005)Google Scholar
  7. 7.
    Theunissen, E.: Integrated Design of a Man-Machine Interface for 4-D Navigation. Delft University Press, The Netherlands (1997)Google Scholar
  8. 8.
    Deutsch, S., Pew, R.: Examining new flight deck technology using human performance modeling. In: Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society 48th Annual Meeting, pp. 108–112. Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, Santa Monica (2004)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. 9.
    Wolfe, J.: What can 1 million trials tell us about visual search? Psychol. Sci. 9(1), 33–39 (1998)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. 10.
    Caroux, L., LeBigot, L., Vibert, N.: Impact of the motion and visual complexity of the background on players’ performance in video game-like displays. Ergonomics 56(12), 1863–1876 (2013)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. 11.
    Neisser, U., Becklen, R.: Selective looking: attending to visually specified events. Cogn. Psychol. 7, 480–494 (1975)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. 12.
    Fischer, E., Haines, R.F., Price, T.A.: Cognitive Issues in Head-Up Displays, NASA Technical Paper 1711. NASA Ames Research Centre, Moffett Field (1980)Google Scholar
  13. 13.
    Stedmon, A.W., Kalawsky, R.S., Hill, K., Cook, C.A.: Old theories, new technologies: cumulative clutter effects using augmented reality. In: IEEE International Conference on Information Visualisation 1999: International Conference on Computer Visualisation, 14–16 July, London, U.K. (1999)Google Scholar
  14. 14.
    Wolfe, J.: Visual attention. In: De Valois, K.K. (ed.) Seeing, 2nd edn, pp. 335–386. Academic Press, San Diego (2000)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. 15.
    Grissinger, M.: Inattentional blindness: what captures your attention? Pharm. Ther. 37(10), 542–555 (2012)Google Scholar
  16. 16.
    Simons, D.: Current approaches to change blindness. Vis. Cogn. 7(1–3), 1–15 (2000)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. 17.
    Rensink, R.: The dynamic representation of scenes. Vis. Cogn. 7, 17–42 (2000)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. 18.
    Rensink, R., O’Regan, J.K., Clark, J.: To see or not to see: the need for attention to perceive changes in scenes. Psychol. Sci. 8(5), 368–373 (1997)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. 19.
    Simons, D., Levin, D.: Failure to detect changes to people during a real-world interaction. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 5(4), 644–649 (1998)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. 20.
    Henderson, J., Hollingworth, A.: Global transsaccadic change blindness during scene perception. Psychol. Sci. 14(5), 493–497 (2003)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. 21.
    Caroux, L., LeBigot, L., Vibert, N.: Impairment of shooting performance by background complexity and motion. Exp. Psychol. 62(2), 98–109 (2015)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. 22.
    Wolfe, J., Oliva, A., Horowitz, T.S., Butcher, S.J., Bompas, A.: Segmentation of objects from backgrounds in visual search tasks. Vis. Res. 42, 2985–3004 (2002)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. 23.
    DEFSTAN 00-25: Human Factors for Designers of Equipment. Crown Copyright: Ministry of Defence Directorate of Standardization, Glasgow (1992)Google Scholar
  24. 24.
    Mumaw, R., Sarter, N., Wickens, C.: Analysis of pilots’ monitoring and performance on an automated flight deck. In: 11th International Symposium on Aviation Psychology, Ohio State (2001)Google Scholar
  25. 25.
    Rensink, R.: When good observers go bad: change blindness, inattentional blindness and visual experience. Psyche: Interdisc. J. Res. Conscious. 6(9) (2000). http://cogprints.org/1050/3/psyche-6-09-rensink.pdf. GoogleScholar. Accessed 09 Feb 2018
  26. 26.
    Rensink, R.: Visual search for change: a probe into the nature of attentional processing. Vis. Cogn. 7, 345–376 (2000)MathSciNetCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. 27.
    Fitts, P.M., Posner, M.I.: Human Performance. Brooks/Cole Publishing Co., Belmont (1967)Google Scholar
  28. 28.
    Ericsson, K., Lehmann, A.: Expert and exceptional performance: evidence of maximal adaption to task constraints. Ann. Psychol. Rev. 47, 272–305 (1996)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. 29.
    Durlach, P.: Change blindness and its implications for complex monitoring and control systems design and operator training. Hum.-Comput. Inter. 19, 423–451 (2004)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. 30.
    Pattison, M., Stedmon, A.W.: Inclusive design and human factors: designing mobile phones for older users. PsychNology J. 4(3), 267–284 (2006)Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Centre for Mobility and TransportCoventry UniversityCoventryUK

Personalised recommendations