What Does the Cognitive Science of Religion Explain?
Abstract
Over the past 15 years or so, the number of empirical projects in the cognitive science of religion (CSR) has grown exponentially and so too has the amount of attention paid to the field, including questions about what the cognitive science of religion is, how it conceptualizes religion and what it explains. The aim of this chapter is to contribute to these discussions by outlining the main objectives of CSR and the assumptions underlying the field. In particular, CSR has often been criticized for not engaging in extensive debates about what religion is. In this chapter I focus extensively on how CSR scholars construe religion and why they have eschewed these definitional debates in favor of engaging in empirical research. In what follows, I discuss how CSR conceptualizes religion, and how this differs from other approaches. Next, I consider how this conceptualization of religion shapes how scholars study it. Finally, I consider the question of how CSR actually explains religion.
Keywords
Cognitive science of religion Explanation of religion Fractionation of religion Traditional study of religionNotes
Acknowledgements
Release time to write this chapter was provided by the College of Humanities at California State University, Northridge. Thanks to Joseph Langston, Justin Barrett , Justin McBrayer, Mitch Hodge and Robert McCauley for reading and providing feedback on an earlier version of this chapter. Thanks also to Paul Parrett for proof reading and editing the first draft.
References
- Astuti, R. 2007. Ancestors and the afterlife. Quaderns de l’Institut Catalàd’Antropologia 23: 61–78.Google Scholar
- Astuti, R., and P.L. Harris. 2008. Understanding mortality and the life of the ancestors in rural Madagascar. Cognitive Science 32: 713–740.Google Scholar
- Atkinson, Quentin D., and Harvey Whitehouse. 2011. The cultural Morphospace of ritual form: Examining modes of religiosity cross-culturally. Evolution and Human Behavior 32: 50–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Atran, Scott. 1998. Folk biology and the anthropology of science: Cognitive Universala and cultural particulars. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21: 547–569.Google Scholar
- ———. 2002. In gods we trust: The evolutionary landscape of religion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
- ———. 2010. Talking to the enemy: Faith, brotherhood, and the (un)making of terrorists. New York: Harper Collins Publishers.Google Scholar
- Banerjee, K., and Paul Bloom. 2015. “Everything happens for a reason”: Children’s beliefs about purpose in life events. Child Development 86: 503–518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- ———. 2014. Why did this happen to me? Religious believers’ and non-believers’ teleological reasoning about life events. Cognition 133: 277–303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Baron-Cohen, S. 1997. Mindblindness: An essay on Aautism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
- Barrett, Justin L., and Rebekah A. Richert. 2003. Anthropomorphism or preparedness? Exploring children’s god concepts. Review of Religious Research 44: 300–312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Barrett, Justin L. 2000. Exploring the natural foundations of religion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4: 1–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- ———. 2001. How ordinary cognition informs petitionary prayer. Journal of Cognition and Culture 1: 259–269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- ———. 2004. Why would anyone believe in god? Walnut Creek: Altamira Press.Google Scholar
- ———. 2007a. Cognitive science of religion: What is it and why is it? Religion Compass 1: 768–786.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- ———. 2007b. Is the spell really broken? Biopsychological explanations of religion and theistic belief. Theology and Science 5: 57–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- ———. 2011. Cognitive science of religion: Looking back, looking forward. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 50: 229–239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Barrett, Justin L., and Melanie A. Nyhof. 2001. Spreading non-natural concepts: The role of intuitive conceptual structures in memory and transmission of cultural materials. Journal of Cognition and Culture 1: 69–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Baumard, Nicolas, and Pascal Boyer. 2013. Explaining moral religions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17: 272–280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bering, Jesse. 2002. Intuitive conceptions of dead agents’ minds: The natural foundations of afterlife beliefs as phenomenological boundary. Journal of Cognition and Culture 2: 263–308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bering, Jesse, and David F. Bjorklund. 2004. The natural emergence of reasoning about the afterlife as a developmental regularity. Developmental Psychology 40: 217–234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bering, Jesse, Carlos Hernandez Blasi, and David F. Bjorklund. 2005. The development of ‘afterlife’ beliefs in religiously and secularly schooled children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 23: 587–607.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bering, Jesse M. 2006. The folk psychology of souls. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 29: 453–462.Google Scholar
- Bering, Jesse, J. Piazza, and G. Ingram. 2011. Princess Alice is watching you: Children’s belief in an invisible person inhibits cheating. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 109: 311–320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bloom, Paul. 2004. Descartes’ baby: How the science of child development explains what makes us human. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
- ———. 2009. Religious belief as an evolutionary accident. In The believing primate, ed. Michael Murray and Jeffrey Schloss, 118–127. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
- Boyer, Pascal. 1994. Cognitive constraints on cultural representations: Natural ontologies and religious ideas. In Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in culture and cognition, ed. L.A. Hirschfield and S. Gelman, 391–411. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- ———. 2003. Religious thought and behaviour as by-products of brain function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7: 119–125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- ———. 2013. Explaining religious concepts. Levi- Strauss the brilliant and problematic ancestor. In Mental culture, classical social theory and the cognitive science of religion, ed. Dimitri Xygalatas and W. M. McCorkle, 164–175. Durham:Acumen.Google Scholar
- Boyer, Pascal, and Pierre Liénard. 2006. Precaution systems and ritualized behavior. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 29: 635–641.Google Scholar
- Brüne, M., and U. Brüne-Cohrs. 2006. Theory of mind—evolution, ontogeny, brain mechanisms and psychopathology. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews 30: 437–455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bulbulia, J., R. Sosis, E. Harris, R. Genet, C. Genet, and K. Wyman. 2008. The evolution of religion: Studies, theories, and critiques. Santa Margarita: Collins Foundation Press.Google Scholar
- Cohen, E., E. Burdett, N. Knight, and Justin L. Barrett. 2011. Cross-cultural similarities and differences in person-body reasoning: Experimental evidence from the UK and Brazilian Amazon. Cognitive Science 35: 1285–1304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Cohen, E., R. Mundry, and S. Kirschner. 2014. Religion, synchrony and cooperation. Religion, Brain & Behavior 4: 20–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Cohen, Emma. 2007. The mind possessed: The cognition of spirit possession in an Afro-Brazilian religious tradition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- ———. 2008. ‘Out with religion’: A novel framing of the religion debate. Manchester: Manchester University Press.Google Scholar
- Cohen, Emma, and Justin L. Barrett. 2008. When minds migrate. Conceptualizing spirit possession. Journal of Cognition and Culture 8: 23–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Dawes, Greg, and James Maclaurin. 2012. What is religion? In A new science of religion, ed. Greg Dawes and James Maclaurin, 11–25. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
- Dawkins, Richard. 2007. The God delusion. London: Black swanGoogle Scholar
- De Cruz, Helen. 2014. Cognitive science of religion and the study of theological concepts. Topoi 33: 487–497.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- ———. 2012. Religious concepts as structured imagination. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion 23: 63–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- De Cruz, Helen, and Johan De Smedt. 2015. A natural history of natural theology. In The cognitive science of theology and philosophy of religion. Cambridge/London: MIT Press.Google Scholar
- Dennett, Daniel Clement. 2006. Breaking the spell: Religion as a Natural Phenomenon. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
- Emmons, N.A., and Deborah Kelemen. 2014. The development of children’s prelife reasoning. Evidence from two cultures. Child Development 85: 1617–1633.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Evans, E. Margaret. 2001. Cognitive and contextual factors in the emergence of diverse belief systems: Creation versus evolution. Cognitive Psychology 42: 217–266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Evans, J.S.B. 2008. Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology 59: 255–278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Fischer, Ronald, Dimitris Xygalatas, Panagiotis Mitkidis, Paul Reddish, Penny Tok, Ivana Konvalinka, and Joseph Bulbulia. 2014. The fire-walker’s high: Affect and physiological responses in an extreme collective ritual. PLoS One 9: e88355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Franke, J. 2014. Has the cognitive science of religion (re) defined ‘Religion’?. Feedback 22.Google Scholar
- Geertz, Armin W. 2015. Evolution of religious belief. In International encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sciences, 2nd ed. Oxford: Elsevier.Google Scholar
- Gelman, S.A., and C.H. Legare. 2011. Concepts and folk theories. Annual Review of Anthropology 40: 379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Gervais, Will, and Joseph Henrich. 2010. The Zeus problem: Why representational content biases cannot explain faith in gods. Journal of Cognition and Culture 10: 383–389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Gervais, Will, Aijana K. Willard, Ara Norenzayan, and Joseph Henrich. 2011. The cultural transmission of faith: Why innate intuitions are necessary, but insufficient, to explain religious belief. Religion 41: 389–410.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Guthrie, Stewart Elliott. 1980. A cognitive theory of religion. Current Anthropology 21: 181–203.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- ———. 1993. Faces in the clouds: A new theory of religion. New York/Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
- Hamilton, W.D. 1964. The genetic evolution of social behaviour II. Journal of Theoretical Biology 7: 17–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Henrich, J., and R. McElreath. 2003. The evolution of cultural evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews 12: 123–135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Heywood, B.T., and Jesse M. Bering. 2014. “Meant to be”: How religious beliefs and cultural religiosity affect the implicit Bias to think teleologically. Religion, Brain & Behavior 4: 183–201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Järnefelt, E., C.F. Canfield, and Deborah Kelemen. 2015. The divided mind of a disbeliever: Intuitive beliefs about nature as purposefully created among different groups of non-religious adults. Cognition 140: 72–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Jensen, J. 2014. What is religion? Routledge: London.Google Scholar
- Johnson, Dominic, and Jesse Bering. 2006. 'Hand of god, mind of man: Punishment and cognition in the evolution of cooperation. Evolutionary Psychology 4: 147470490600400119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Johnson, Dominic D.P. 2005. God’s punishment and public goods. Human Nature 16: 410–446.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Jong, Jonathan. 2012. Explaining religion (Away?) theism and the cognitive science of religion. Sophia 52: 521–533.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kahneman, Daniel. 2012. Thinking, fast and slow. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
- Kelemen, Deborah. 2004. Are children “intuitive theists”?: Reasoning about purpose and design in nature. Psychological Science 15: 295–301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kelemen, Deborah, and C. DiYanni. 2005. Intuitions about origins: Purpose and intelligent design in children’s reasoning about nature. Journal of Cognition and Development 6: 3–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Knight, N. 2008. Yukatek Maya children’s attributions of belief to natural and non-natural entities. Journal of Cognition and Culture 8: 235–243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Lanman, Jonathan. 2012. On the non-evolution of atheism and the importance of definitions and data. Religion, Brain & Behavior 2: 76–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Lanman, Jonathan A., and Michael D. Buhrmester. 2016. Religious actions speak louder than words: Exposure to credibility-enhancing displays predicts theism. Religion, Brain & Behavior 7: 1–14.Google Scholar
- Lawson, E. Thomas. 2000. Towards a cognitive science of religion. Numen 47: 338–348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Lawson, E. Thomas, and Robert N. McCauley. 1990. Rethinking religion: Connecting cognition and culture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
- Leech, David, and Aku Visala. 2011. The cognitive science of religion: Implications for theism? Zygon 46: 47–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Legare, C.H., E.M. Evans, K.S. Rosengren, and P.L. Harris. 2012. The coexistence of natural and supernatural explanations across cultures and development. Child Development 83: 779–793.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Legare, C.H., and S.A. Gelman. 2008. Bewitchment, biology or both: The co-existence of natural and supernatural explanatory frameworks across development. Cognitive Science 32: 607–642.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Legare, Cristine H., and Rachel E. Watson-Jones. 2015. The evolution and ontogeny of ritual. In The handbook of evolutionary psychology, 829–847. Hoboken: Wiley.Google Scholar
- Lindeman, M., and A.M. Svedholm. 2012. What’s in a term? Paranormal, superstitious, magical and supernatural beliefs by any other name would mean the same. Review of General Psychology 16: 241–255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Malley, B. 2004. How the Bible works: An anthropological study of evangelical biblicism. Walnut Creek: Altamira Press.Google Scholar
- McCauley, Robert N. (in press) Philosophical Foundations of the Cognitive Science of Religion. Bloomsbury Academic: London: UK.Google Scholar
- ———. 2011. Why religion is natural and science is not. Oxford: New York University Press.Google Scholar
- McCauley, Robert N., and Emma Cohen. 2010. Cognitive science and the naturalness of religion. Philosophy Compass 5: 779–792.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- McCauley, Robert N., and E. Thomas Lawson. 2002. Bringing ritual to mind: Psychological foundations of cultural forms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- McCutcheon, R.T. 1997. Manufacturing religion: The discourse on Sui generis religion and the politics of Nostalgie. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
- McKay, R., C. Efferson, H. Whitehouse, and E. Fehr. 2011. Wrath of god: Religious primes and punishment. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 278: 1858–1863.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Mckay, R., and H. Whitehouse. 2014. Religion and morality. Psychological Bulletin 141: 447–473.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Murray, Michael J. 2008. Four arguments that the cognitive psychology of religion undermines the justification of religious belief. In The evolution of religion: Studies, theories, and critiques, ed. J. Bulbulia, R. Sosis, E. Harris, R. Genet, C. Genet, and K. Wyman, 393–398. Santa Margarita: Collins Foundation Press.Google Scholar
- Nicholson, H. 2016. The spirit of contradiction in Christianity and Buddhism. Oxford: Oxford University Pres.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Norenzayan, Ara. 2013. Big gods: How religion transformed cooperation and conflict. Princeton/New Jersey: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
- Norenzayan, Ara, Will Gervais, and K.H. Trzesniewski. 2012. Mentalizing deficits constrain belief in a personal god. PLoS One 7: e36880.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Norenzayan, Ara, and A.F. Shariff. 2008. The origin and evolution of religious prosociality. Science 322: 58–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Norenzayan, Ara, Azim F. Shariff, Will M. Gervais, Aiyana K. Willard, Rita A. McNamara, Edward Slingerland & Joseph Henrich. 2014. The cultural evolution of prosocial religions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1–86. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x14001356
- Purzycki, B.G.. 2013. The minds of gods: A comparative study of supernatural agency. Cognition 129: 163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Richerson, P.J., and R. Boyd. 2008. Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
- Richert, R.A., and P.L. Harris. 2006. The ghost in my body: Children’s developping concept of the soul. Journal of Cognition and Culture 6: 409–427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Shtulman, A., and K. Harrington. 2016. Tensions between science and intuition across lifespan. Topics in Cognitive Science 8: 117–137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Slingerland, Edward. 2008. Who is afraid of reductionism? The study of religion in the age of cognitive science. Journal of the American Academy of Religion 76: 375–411.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Slingerland, Edward, and J. Bulbulia. 2011. Introductory essay: Evolutionary science and the study of religion. Religion 41: 307–328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Slingerland, Edward, and Bruce M. Sullivan. 2017. Durkheim with data: The database of religious history. Journal of the American Academy of Religion. 85 (2): 312–347CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Smith, J.Z. 2004. Relating religion: Essays in the study of religion. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
- Sorensen, J. 2007. A cognitive theory of magic. Plymouth: Rowman Altamira.Google Scholar
- Sosis, Richard. 2009. The adaptationist—by-product debate on the evolution of religion: Five misunderstandings of the adaptationist program. Journal of Cognition and Culture 9: 315–332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Sosis, Richard, and E.R. Bressler. 2003. Cooperation and commune longevity: A test of the costly signaling theory of religion. Cross-Cultural Research 37: 211–239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Swann, W.B., Jr., M.D. Buhrmester, A. Gómez, J. Jetten, B. Bastian, A. Vázquez, and G. Finchilescu. 2014. What makes a group worth dying for? Identity fusion fosters perception of family ties, promoting self-sacrifice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 106: 912–926.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Taves, Ann. 2011. Religious experience reconsidered: A building-block approach to the study of religion and other special things. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
- Trigg, Roger, and Justin L. Barrett. 2014. The roots of religion. Exploring the cognitive science of religion. Surrey: Ashgate.Google Scholar
- Turchin, P., H. Whitehouse, P. Francois, E. Slingerland, and M. Collard. 2012. A historical database of sociocultural evolution. Cliodynamics. The Journal of Theoretical and Mathematical History 3: 271–273.Google Scholar
- Ward, T.B. 1994. Structured imagination: The role of category structure in exemplar generation. Cognitive Psychology 27: 1–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Watson-Jones, R. E., H. Whitehouse, and C. H. Legare. in press. In-group ostracism increases high fidelity imitation in early childhood. Psychological Science.Google Scholar
- White, Claire. 2016a. The cognitive foundations of reincarnation. Method & Theory in the Study of Religion 28: 1–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- ———. 2016b. Cross-cultural similarities in reasoning about personal continuity in reincarnation: Evidence from South India. Religion, Brain and Behavior. 6: 130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- ———. 2017. What the cognitive science of religion is (and is not). In Theory in a time of excess—The case of the academic study of religion, ed. Aaron W. Hughes. London: Equinox Publishing.Google Scholar
- White, Claire, M. Marin, and M.T. Fessler. 2017. Not just dead meat: An evolutionary account of corpse treatment in mortuary rituals. Journal of Cognition and Culture 17: 146–168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Whitehouse, Harvey. 1995. Inside the cult: Religious innovation and transmission in Papua New Guinea. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
- ———. 2004. Modes of religiosity: A cognitive theory of religious transmission. Rowman Altamira.Google Scholar
- ———. 2008. Cognitive evolution and religion; cognition and religious evolution. Issues in Ethnology and Anthropology, 3.Google Scholar
- Willard, Aiyana K., and Ara Norenzayan. 2013. Cognitive biases explain religious belief, paranormal belief and belief in life’s purpose. Cognition 129: 379–391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Wilson, David Sloan. 2002. Darwin’s Cathedral: Evolution, religion, and the nature of society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
- Wittgenstein, L. (2001). Philosophical investigations. Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
- Xygalatas, Dimitri, I. Konvalinka, J. Bulbulia, and A. Roepstorff. 2011. Quantifying collective effervescence: Heart-rate dynamics at a fire-walking ritual. Communicative & Integrative Biology 4: 735–738.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Xygalatas, Dimitri, and W.M. McCorkle. 2013. Mental culture: Classical social theory and the cognitive science of religion. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar