Advertisement

Functions of Anger in the Emotion System

  • Ira J. Roseman
Chapter

Abstract

This chapter considers the functions of anger as an emotion within an often functional emotion system. It is proposed that emotions are general-purpose coping strategies, usually comprising phenomenological, physiological, expressive, behavioral, and emotivational goal components, each of which fulfills specific functions within an emotion’s strategy. For example, typical instances of anger involve thoughts about undeserved harm, feeling hot and ready to explode, activity in circuits running through the medial amygdala and hypothalamus, lowered brows and squarish mouth, readiness to attack, and a goal of hurting its target or compelling change in the target’s behavior. Together they implement a strategy of interpersonal coercion. Emotions are typically elicited by combinations of appraisals about significant changes in motive-attainment (e.g., goal blockage caused by other persons, when there may be something that can be done about it, eliciting anger) and function to provide alternative ways to attain one’s motives (alternative to each other and to action governed by what have traditionally been considered motives, such as hunger and the need for achievement) in particular types of situations. The Emotion System theory offers an account of why people and other organisms have emotions and why they have the particular emotions that they do. Explanations for emotion dysfunctions, such as anger disorders, are also discussed. Finally, the theory is applied to examine anger in the political domain.

Keywords

Anger Emotions Coping Cognitive appraisal Emotion system Motivation 

References

  1. American Heritage Dictionary (n.d.). Coerce. Retrieved from https://ahdictionary.com/word/search.html?q=coerce.
  2. American National Election Studies. (2016). ANES 2016 time series study. Ann Arbor, MI: Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research.Google Scholar
  3. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Amsel, A. (1992). Frustration theory: An analysis of dispositional learning and memory. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. (2002). Human aggression. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 27–51.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Arceneaux, K., & Nicholson, S. P. (2012). Who wants to have a tea party? The who, what, and why of the tea party movement. Political Science & Politics, 45, 700–710.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Aristotle (1966). Rhetoric. In Aristotle's Rhetoric and Poetics (W. R. Roberts, Trans.). New York, NY: Modern Library. (Original work published 350 B.C.)Google Scholar
  8. Arnold, M. B. (1960). Emotion and personality. New York, NY: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
  9. Averill, J. R. (1980). A constructivist view of emotion. In R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (Eds.), Emotion: Theory, research and experience (Vol. 1, pp. 305–339). New York, NY: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Averill, J. R. (1982). Anger and aggression: An essay on emotion. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Ax, A. F. (1953). The physiological differentiation between fear and anger in humans. Psychosomatic Medicine, 15, 433–442.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Banks, A. J. (2014). The public’s anger: White racial attitudes and opinions toward health care reform. Political Behavior, 36, 493–514.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Banks, A. J. (2016). Are group cues necessary? How anger makes ethnocentrism among whites a stronger predictor of racial and immigration policy opinions. Political Behavior, 38, 635–657.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Barrett, L. F. (2009). Variety is the spice of life: A psychological construction approach to understanding variability in emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 1284–1306.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Barrett, L. F., Mesquita, B., Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2007). The experience of emotion. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 373–403.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Berkowitz, L. (2012). A different view of anger: The cognitive-neoassociation conception of the relation of anger to aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 38, 322–333.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Blair, R. J. (2016). The neurobiology of impulsive aggression. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 26, 4–9.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Blanchard, D. C., & Blanchard, R. J. (2003). What can animal aggression research tell us about human aggression? Hormones and Behavior, 44, 171–177.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss (Vol. 1: Attachment). New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  20. Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss (Vol. 2: Separation). New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  21. Bozman, A. W., & Beck, J. G. (1991). Covariation of sexual desire and sexual arousal: The effects of anger and anxiety. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 20, 47–60.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Brown, R. P., Wohl, M. J., & Exline, J. J. (2008). Taking up offenses: Secondhand forgiveness and group identification. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 1406–1419.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Buss, J., Havel, P. J., Epel, E., Lin, J., Blackburn, E., & Daubenmier, J. (2014). Associations of ghrelin with eating behaviors, stress, metabolic factors, and telomere length among overweight and obese women: Preliminary evidence of attenuated ghrelin effects in obesity? Appetite, 76, 84–94.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2012). Cybernetic control processes and the self-regulation of behavior. In R. M. Ryan (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of human motivation (pp. 28–42). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  25. Ceulemans, E., Kuppens, P., & Mechelen, I. V. (2012). Capturing the structure of distinct types of individual differences in the situation-specific experience of emotions: The case of anger. European Journal of Personality, 26, 484–495.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Cheung-Blunden, V., & Blunden, B. (2008). The emotional construal of war: Anger, fear, and other negative emotions. Peace and Conflict: Journal of Peace Psychology, 14, 123–150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Clark-Polner, E., Wager, T. D., Satpute, A. B., & Barrett, L. F. (2016). Neural fingerprinting: Meta-analysis, variation and the search for brain-based essences in the science of emotion. In L. F. Barrett, M. Lewis, & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (4th ed., pp. 146–165). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  28. Cloninger, S. C., & Leibo, S. A. (2017). Understanding angry groups. Santa Barbara, CA: Praeger.Google Scholar
  29. Coulson, M. (2004). Attributing emotion to static body postures: Recognition accuracy, confusions, and viewpoint dependence. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 28, 117–139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Davies, J. C. (1962). Toward a theory of revolution. American Sociological Review, 27, 5–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Davitz, J. R. (1969). The language of emotion. New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  32. de Rivera, J. H. (1977). A structural theory of the emotions. Psychological issues monograph 40. New York, NY: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
  33. de Vos, B., van Zomeren, M., Gordijn, E. H., & Postmes, T. (2013). The communication of “pure” group-based anger reduces tendencies toward intergroup conflict because it increases out-group empathy. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39, 1043–1052.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. de Vos, B., van Zomeren, M., Gordijn, E. H., & Postmes, T. (2016). When does the communication of group-based anger increase outgroup empathy in intergroup conflict? The role of perceived procedural unfairness and outgroup consensus. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations. Retrieved from  https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430216674340.
  35. DiGiuseppe, R., & Tafrate, R. C. (2007). Understanding anger disorders. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  36. Dodge, K. A. (2006). Translational science in action: Hostile attributional style and the development of aggressive behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology, 18, 791–814.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effects of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychological Review, 66, 183–201.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1989). Human ethology. New York, NY: Aldine De Gruyter.Google Scholar
  39. Ekman, P. (1972). Universals and cultural differences in facial expressions of emotion. In J. K. Cole (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation, 1971 (Vol. 19, pp. 207–283). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
  40. Ekman, P. (2003). Emotions revealed. New York, NY: Holt.Google Scholar
  41. Ekman, P., Friesen, W. V., & Hager, J. C. (2002). Facial action coding system: The manual. Salt Lake City, UT: A Human Face.Google Scholar
  42. Ekman, P., Levenson, R. W., & Friesen, W. V. (1983). Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishes among emotions. Science, 221, 1208–1210.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Elliot, A. J., & Church, M. A. (1997). A hierarchical model of approach and avoidance achievement motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 218–232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Ellsworth, P. C., & Tong, E. M. W. (2006). What does it mean to be angry at yourself? Categories, appraisals, and the problem of language. Emotion, 6, 572–586.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Fehr, B., & Russell, J. A. (1984). Concept of emotion viewed from a prototype perspective. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 464–486.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Feldman, R. A. (1969). Group integration and intense interpersonal disliking. Human Relations, 22, 405–413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Fernandez, E., & Johnson, S. L. (2016). Anger in psychological disorders: Prevalence, presentation, etiology and prognostic implications. Clinical Psychology Review, 46, 124–135.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Fernandez, E., & Wasan, A. (2010). The anger of pain sufferers: Attributions to agents and appraisals of wrongdoings. In M. Potegal, G. Stemmler, & C. Spielberger (Eds.), International handbook of anger (pp. 449–464). New York, NY: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Fessler, D. M. (2010). Madmen: An evolutionary perspective on anger and men’s violent responses to transgression. In M. Potegal, G. Stemmler, & C. Spielberger (Eds.), International handbook of anger (pp. 361–381). New York, NY: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Fischer, A. H., & Manstead, A. S. R. (2016). Social functions of emotion and emotion regulation. In L. F. Barrett, M. Lewis, & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (4th ed., pp. 424–439). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  51. Fischer, A. H., & Roseman, I. J. (2007). Beat them or ban them: The characteristics and social functions of anger and contempt. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 103–115.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Fitness, J., & Fletcher, G. J. O. (1993). Love, hate, anger and jealousy in close relationships: A prototype and cognitive appraisal analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 942–958.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. French, J. R. P., & Raven, B. (1959). The bases of social power. In D. Cartwright & A. Zander (Eds.), Group dynamics (pp. 150–167). New York, NY: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
  54. Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  55. Frijda, N. H. (1994). The Lex Talionis: On vengeance. In S. H. Van Goozen, N. E. Van de Poll, & J. A. Sergeant (Eds.), Emotions: Essays on emotion theory (pp. 263–289). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  56. Frijda, N. H., Kuipers, P., & ter Schure, E. (1989). Relations among emotion, appraisal, and emotional action readiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 212–228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Frijda, N. H., Ortony, A., Sonnemans, J., & Clore, G. L. (1992). The complexity of intensity: Issues concerning the structure of emotion intensity. In M. S. Clark (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 13, pp. 60–89). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  58. Gable, P. A., Poole, B. D., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2015). Anger perceptually and conceptually narrows cognitive scope. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(1), 163–174.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Garrett, K. N., & Bankert, A. (2018). The moral roots of partisan division: How moral conviction heightens affective polarization. British Journal of Political Science,1-20.  https://doi.org/10.1017/S000712341700059X.
  60. Gibson, D. E., & Callister, R. R. (2010). Anger in organizations: Review and integration. Journal of Management, 36, 66–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Goldstein, H. S., Edelberg, R., Meier, C. F., & Davis, L. (1989). Relationship of expressed anger to forearm muscle vascular resistance. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 33(4), 497–504.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Gollwitzer, M., Meder, M., & Schmitt, M. (2011). What gives victims satisfaction when they seek revenge? European Journal of Social Psychology, 41, 364–374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Gramarist.com (n.d.) Compel vs. impel. retrieved from http://grammarist.com/usage/compel-impel/
  64. Green, J. A., Whitney, P. G., & Gustafson, G. E. (2010). Vocal expressions of anger. In M. Potegal, G. Stemmler, & C. Spielberger (Eds.), International handbook of anger (pp. 139–156). New York, NY: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Griner, L. A., & Smith, C. A. (2000). Contributions of motivational orientation to appraisal and emotion. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 727–740.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Groenendyk, E. W., & Banks, A. J. (2014). Emotional rescue: How affect helps partisans overcome collective action problems. Political Psychology, 35, 359–378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L. (1994). Emotional contagion. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  68. Hill, P. C., Pargament, K. I., Hood, R. W., McCullough, M. E., Jr., Sawyers, J. P., Larson, D. B., & Zinnbauer, B. J. (2000). Conceptualizing religion and spirituality: Points of commonality, points of departure. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 30, 51–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Hochschild, A. R. (2016). Strangers in their own land: Anger and mourning on the American right. New York, NY: The New Press.Google Scholar
  70. Huddy, L. (2013). From group identity to political cohesion and commitment. In L. Huddy, D. O. Sears, & J. S. Levy (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of political psychology (pp. 737–773). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Huddy, L., Mason, L., & Aarøe, L. (2015). Expressive partisanship: Campaign involvement, political emotion, and partisan identity. American Political Science Review, 109, 1–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Izard, C. E. (1983). The maximally discriminative facial movement coding system (MAX) (Rev. ed.). Newark, DE: Information Technologies and University Media Services, University of Delaware.Google Scholar
  73. Izard, C. E. (1991). The psychology of emotions. New York, NY: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Izard, C. E. (1993). Four systems for emotion activation: Cognitive and noncognitive processes. Psychological Review, 100, 68–90.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Jahoda, G. (2012). Critical reflections on some recent definitions of “culture”. Culture & Psychology, 18, 289–303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Janoff-Bulman, R. (1979). Characterological versus behavioral self-blame: Inquiries into depression and rape. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1798–1809.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Jasper, J. M. (2014). Constructing indignation: Anger dynamics in protest movements. Emotion Review, 6, 208–213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Jing, J., Gillette, R., & Weiss, K. R. (2009). Evolving concepts of arousal: Insights from simple model systems. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 20, 405–428.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2004). Healthy and unhealthy emotion regulation: Personality processes, individual differences, and life span development. Journal of Personality, 72, 1301–1334.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  80. Johnston, G., Roseman, I., & Katz, S. (2014). Discrete emotions mediate perceptions of presidential candidates: A study using a nationally representative sample. Poster session presented at the 26th annual meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association, Boston, MA.Google Scholar
  81. Kassinove, H. (1995). Anger disorders: Definition, diagnosis, and treatment. Washington, DC: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
  82. Kassinove, H., Sukhodolsky, D. G., Tsytsarev, S. V., & Solovyova, S. (1997). Self-reported anger episodes in Russia and America. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 12(2), 301–324.Google Scholar
  83. Keltner, D., & Haidt, J. (2001). Social functions of emotions. In T. Mayne & G. Bonanno (Eds.), Emotions: Current issues and future directions (pp. 192–213). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  84. Keltner, D., Tracy, J., Sauter, D. A., Cordaro, D. C., & McNeil, G. (2016). Expression of emotion. In L. F. Barrett, M. Lewis, & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (4th ed., pp. 467–482). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  85. Keltner, D., Young, R. C., Heerey, E. A., Oemig, C., & Monarch, N. D. (1998). Teasing in hierarchical and intimate relations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1231–1247.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. Kenrick, D. T., & Keefe, R. C. (1992). Age preferences in mates reflect sex differences in human reproductive strategies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 15(1), 75–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  87. Kerr, M. A., & Schneider, B. H. (2008). Anger expression in children and adolescents: A review of the empirical literature. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 559–577.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  88. Klandermans, B., Van der Toorn, J., & Van Stekelenburg, J. (2008). Embeddedness and identity: How immigrants turn grievances into action. American Sociological Review, 73, 992–1012.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Kleinginna, P., Jr., & Kleinginna, A. (1981). A categorized list of emotion definitions, with suggestions for a consensual definition. Motivation and Emotion, 5, 345–379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Kötter, R., & Stephan, K. Ε. (1997). Useless or helpful? The" limbic system" concept. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 8, 139–145.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  91. Kövecses, Z. (2010). Cross-cultural experience of anger: A psycholinguistic analysis. In M. Potegal, G. Stemmler, & C. Spielberger (Eds.), International handbook of anger (pp. 157–174). New York, NY: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  92. Kreibig, S. D. (2010). Autonomic nervous system activity in emotion: A review. Biological Psychology, 84, 394–421.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  93. Kuppens, P., Van Mechelen, I., Smits, D. J., & De Boeck, P. (2003). The appraisal basis of anger: Specificity, necessity and sufficiency of components. Emotion, 3, 254–269.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  94. Lambert, A. J., Scherer, L. D., Schott, J. P., Olson, K. R., Andrews, R. K., O'Brien, T. C., & Zisser, A. R. (2010). Rally effects, threat, and attitude change: An integrative approach to understanding the role of emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 886–903.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  95. Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  96. Lemay, E. P., Jr., Overall, N. C., & Clark, M. S. (2012). Experiences and interpersonal consequences of hurt feelings and anger. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103, 982–1006.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. Lench, H. C., & Levine, L. J. (2008). Goals and responses to failure: Knowing when to hold them and when to fold them. Motivation and Emotion, 32, 127–140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. Lench, H. C., Bench, S. W., Darbor, K. E., & Moore, M. (2015). A functionalist manifesto: Goal-related emotions from an evolutionary perspective. Emotion Review, 7, 90–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  99. Lerner, J. S., Gonzalez, R. M., Small, D. A., & Fischhoff, B. (2003). Effects of fear and anger on perceived risks of terrorism: A national field experiment. Psychological Science, 14, 144–150.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  100. Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2001). Fear, anger, and risk. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 146–159.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  101. Lerner, J. S., & Tiedens, L. Z. (2006). Portrait of the angry decision maker: How appraisal tendencies shape anger's influence on cognition. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 19, 115–137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  102. Lerner, M. J. (2015). Understanding how the justice motive shapes our lives and treatment of one another: Exciting contributions and misleading claims. In R. S. Cropanzano & M. L. Ambrose (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of justice in the workplace(pp. 205-234). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  103. Lewis, M., Alessandri, S. M., & Sullivan, M. W. (1990). Violation of expectancy, loss of control, and anger expressions in young infants. Developmental Psychology, 26, 745–751.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  104. Lickel, B. (2012). Retribution and revenge. In L. R. Tropp (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of intergroup conflict (pp. 89–105). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  105. Litvak, P. M., Lerner, J. S., Tiedens, L. Z., & Shonk, K. (2010). Fuel in the fire: How anger impacts judgment and decision-making. In M. Potegal, G. Stemmler, & C. Spielberger (Eds.), International handbook of anger (pp. 287–310). New York, NY: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  106. Mackie, D. M., Devos, T., & Smith, E. R. (2000). Intergroup emotions: Explaining offensive action tendencies in an intergroup context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 602–616.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. Matsumoto, D., & Hwang, H. S. (2011). Reading facial expressions of emotion. Psychological Science Agenda. Retrieved from http://www.apa.org/science/about/psa/2011/05/facial-expressions.aspx
  108. Matsumoto, D., Hwang, H. C., & Frank, M. G. (2014). Emotions expressed in speeches by leaders of ideologically motivated groups predict aggression. Behavioral Sciences of Terrorism and Political Aggression, 6(1), 1–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  109. Matsumoto, D., Keltner, D., Shiota, M. N., O’Sullivan, M., & Frank, M. (2008). Facial expressions of emotion. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd ed., pp. 211–234). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  110. Musgrove, L., & McGarty, C. (2008). Opinion-based group membership as a predictor of collective emotional responses and support for pro-and anti-war action. Social Psychology, 39, 37–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  111. Nabi, R. (2002). The theoretical versus the lay meaning of disgust: Implications for emotion research. Cognition and Emotion, 16, 695–703.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  112. Northouse, P. G. (2016). Leadership: Theory and practice (7th ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  113. Novaco, R. W. (2010). Anger and psychopathology. In M. Potegal, G. Stemmler, & C. Spielberger (Eds.), International handbook of anger (pp. 465–497). New York, NY: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  114. Ortony, A., Clore, G. L., & Collins, A. (1988). The cognitive structure of emotions. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  115. Panksepp, J. (2017). Instinctual foundations of animal minds: Comparative perspectives on the evolved affective neural substrate of emotions and learned behaviors. In J. Call, Gordon M. Burghardt, I. M. Pepperberg, C. T. Snowdon, & T. Zentall (Eds.). APA handbook of comparative psychology.Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. 1, 475–500.Google Scholar
  116. Parkinson, C., Walker, T. T., Memmi, S., & Wheatley, T. (2017). Emotions are understood from biological motion across remote cultures. Emotion, 17, 459–477.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  117. Pettigrew, T. F. (2017). Social psychological perspectives on Trump supporters. Journal of Social and Political Psychology, 5, 107–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  118. Pew Research Center (2013). Anger at government most pronounced among conservative Republicans. Retrieved from http://www.people-press.org/2013/09/30/anger-at-government-most-pronounced-among-conservative-republicans/.
  119. Pew Research Center (2016). Partisanship and Political Animosity in 2016. retrieved from http://www.people-press.org/2016/06/22/partisanship-and-political-animosity-in-2016/
  120. Plutchik, R. (1980). A general psychoevolutionary theory of emotion. In R. Plutchik & H. Kellerman (Eds.), Emotion: Theory, research, and experience: Vol. 1. Theories of emotion (pp. 3–33). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  121. Potegal, M., & Qiu, P. (2010). Anger in children’s tantrums: A new, quantitative, behaviorally based model. In M. Potegal, G. Stemmler, & C. Spielberger (Eds.), International handbook of anger (pp. 193–217). New York, NY: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  122. Potegal, M., & Stemmler, G. (2010). Cross-disciplinary views of anger: Consensus and controversy. In M. Potegal, G. Stemmler, & C. Spielberger (Eds.), International handbook of anger (pp. 3–7). New York, NY: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  123. Rasmussen Reports (2016). Trump Voters Don’t Like the Feds, Clinton Voters Do. Retrieved from http://www.rasmussenreports.com/public_content/politics/elections/election_2016/trump_voters_don_t_like_the_feds_clinton_voters_do.
  124. Redlawsk, D. P., Roseman, I. J., Mattes, K., & Katz, S. (2018). They’re laughable, they lie: The role of contempt in negative campaigning. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
  125. Redlawsk, D. P., Roseman, I. J., Mattes, K., & Katz, S. (in press). Donald Trump, contempt, and the 2016 GOP Iowa caucuses. Journal of Elections, Public Opinion & Parties.Google Scholar
  126. Reisenzein, R. (2000). The subjective experience of surprise. In H. Bless & J. P. Forgas (Eds.), The message within: The role of subjective experience in social cognition and behavior (pp. 262–279). Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  127. Renshaw, K. D., Blais, R. K., & Smith, T. W. (2010). Components of negative affectivity and marital satisfaction: The importance of actor and partner anger. Journal of Research in Personality, 44, 328–334.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  128. Roseman, I. J. (1984). Cognitive determinants of emotions: A structural theory. In P. Shaver (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 5, pp. 11–36). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
  129. Roseman, I. J. (2001). A model of appraisal in the emotion system: Integrating theory, research, and applications. In K. R. Scherer, A. Schorr, & T. Johnstone (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research (pp. 68–91). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  130. Roseman, I. J. (2008). Motivations and emotivations: Approach, avoidance, and other tendencies in motivated and emotional behavior. In A. J. Elliot (Ed.), Handbook of approach and avoidance motivation (pp. 343–366). New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
  131. Roseman, I. J. (2011). Emotional behaviors, emotivational goals, emotion strategies: Multiple levels of organization integrate variable and consistent responses. Emotion Review, 3, 434–443.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  132. Roseman, I. J. (2013). Appraisal in the emotion system: Coherence in strategies for coping. Emotion Review, 5, 141–149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  133. Roseman, I. J. (2017). Transformative events: Appraisal bases of passion and mixed emotions. Emotion Review, 9, 133–139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  134. Roseman, I. J. (in press). Rejecting the unworthy: The causes, components, and consequences of contempt. In M. Mason (Ed.), The moral psychology of contempt. London: Rowman & Littlefield.Google Scholar
  135. Roseman, I. J., Antoniou, A. A., & Jose, P. E. (1996). Appraisal determinants of emotions: Constructing a more accurate and comprehensive theory. Cognition and Emotion, 10, 241–277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  136. Roseman, I. J., & Fischer, A. H. (2017). Anger, dislike, contempt, and hatred. Manuscript submitted for publication.Google Scholar
  137. Roseman, I. J, Johnston, B. M., Garguilo, S., Floman, J. L., Bryant, A. D., Frazier, I. R., & Nugent, M. K. (2012, May). Are some emotions more politically potent than others? Poster session presented at the 24th Annual Convention, Association for Psychological Science, Chicago, IL.Google Scholar
  138. Roseman, I. J., Redlawsk, D. P., Mattes, K., & Katz, S. (2017, July). “Little Marco,” “Lyin’ Ted,” and “Crooked Hillary”: The power of contempt in American electoral politics and beyond. In I. J. Roseman (Chair) “The power and perils of contempt: advances in understanding.” Symposium presented at the International Society for Research on Emotions biannual conference, St. Louis, MO.Google Scholar
  139. Roseman, I. J., & Smith, C. A. (2001). Appraisal theory: Overview, assumptions, varieties, controversies. In K. R. Scherer, A. Schorr, & T. Johnstone (Eds.), Appraisal processes in emotion: Theory, methods, research (pp. 3–19). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  140. Roseman, I. J., Steele, A., & Goodvin, A. (2017). Anger, contempt, hatred, and dislike. Rutgers University: Unpublished raw data.Google Scholar
  141. Roseman, I. J., Wiest, C., & Swartz, T. S. (1994). Phenomenology, behaviors, and goals differentiate discrete emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 206–221.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  142. Rottenberg, J. E., & Johnson, S. L. (2007). Emotion and psychopathology: Bridging affective and clinical science. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  143. Rutter, M. (2005). Incidence of autism spectrum disorders: Changes over time and their meaning. Acta Paediatrica, 94, 2–15.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  144. Rydell, R. J., Mackie, D. M., Maitner, A. T., Claypool, H. M., Ryan, M. J., & Smith, E. R. (2008). Arousal, processing, and risk taking: Consequences of intergroup anger. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 1141–1152.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  145. Sadler, M. S., Lineberger, M., Correll, J., & Park, B. (2005). Emotions, attributions, and policy endorsement in response to the September 11th terrorist attacks. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 27, 249–258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  146. Schaffner, B. F., MacWilliams, M., & Nteta, T. (2017, January). Explaining White polarization in the 2016 vote for President: The sobering role of racism and sexism. Paper presented at the Conference on The U.S. Presidential Election of 2016: Domestic and International Aspects. IDC Herzliya Campus.Google Scholar
  147. Scherer, K. R. (Ed.). (1988). Facets of emotion: Recent research. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  148. Scherer, K. R. (2009). The dynamic architecture of emotion: Evidence for the component process model. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 1307–1351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  149. Scherer, K. R., & Fontaine, J. J. R. (2013). Driving the emotion process: The appraisal component. In J. J. R. Fontaine, K. R. Scherer, & C. Soriano (Eds.), Components of emotional meaning: A sourcebook (pp. 186–209). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  150. Scherer, K. R., & Wallbott, H. G. (1994). Evidence for universality and cultural variation of differential emotion response patterning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 310–328.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  151. Sell, A., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2014). The human anger face evolved to enhance cues of strength. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35(5), 425–429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  152. Shaver, P. R., Schwartz, J., Kirson, J., & O’Connor, C. (1987). Emotional knowledge: Further explorations of a prototype approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1061–1086.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  153. Simpson, J. A., & Belsky, J. (2008). Attachment theory within a modern evolutionary framework. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (2nd ed., pp. 131–157). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  154. Smetana, J. G., Daddis, C., & Chuang, S. S. (2003). “Clean your room!” A longitudinal investigation of adolescent-parent conflict and conflict resolution in middle-class African American families. Journal of Adolescent Research, 18, 631–650.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  155. Smith, C. A., & Kirby, L. D. (2009). Putting appraisal in context: Toward a relational model of appraisal and emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 23, 1352–1372.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  156. Smith, C. A., & Kirby, L. D. (2011). The role of appraisal and emotion in coping and adaptation. In R. J. Contrada & A. Baum (Eds.), The handbook of stress science: Biology, psychology, and health (pp. 195–208). New York, NY: Springer.Google Scholar
  157. Smith, E. R. (1993). Social identity and social emotions: Toward new conceptualizations of prejudice. In D. M. Mackie & D. L. Hamilton (Eds.), Affect, cognition and stereotyping: Interactive processes in group perception (pp. 297–315). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  158. Solomon, R. C. (1990). A passion for justice. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
  159. Spanovic, M., Lickel, B., Denson, T. F., & Petrovic, N. (2010). Fear and anger as predictors of motivation for intergroup aggression: Evidence from Serbia and Republika Srpska. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 13, 725–739.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  160. Sparks, H. (2015). Mama grizzlies and guardians of the republic: The democratic and intersectional politics of anger in the Tea Party movement. New Political Science, 37, 25–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  161. Stemmler, G. (2010). Somatovisceral activation during anger. In M. Potegal, G. Stemmler, & C. Spielberger (Eds.), International handbook of anger (pp. 103–121). New York, NY: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  162. Strawhun, J., Adams, N., & Huss, M. T. (2013). The assessment of cyberstalking: An expanded examination including social networking, attachment, jealousy, and anger in relation to violence and abuse. Violence and Victims, 28, 715–730.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  163. Stringaris, A., Cohen, P., Pine, D. S., & Leibenluft, E. (2009). Adult outcomes of youth irritability: A 20-year prospective community-based study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 1048–1054.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  164. Sukhodolsky, D. G., Golub, A., & Cromwell, E. N. (2001). Development and validation of the anger rumination scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 31, 689–700.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  165. Tausch, N., & Becker, J. C. (2013). Emotional reactions to success and failure of collective action as predictors of future action intentions: A longitudinal investigation in the context of student protests in Germany. British Journal of Social Psychology, 52, 525–542.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  166. Tausch, N., Becker, J. C., Spears, R., Christ, O., Saab, R., Singh, P., & Siddiqui, R. N. (2011). Explaining radical group behavior: Developing emotion and efficacy routes to normative and nonnormative collective action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, 129–148.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  167. Taylor, S. (1970). Aggressive behavior as a function of approval motivation and physical attack. Psychonomic Science, 18, 195–196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  168. Tolman, E. C. (1923). A behavioristic account of the emotions. Psychological Review, 30, 217–227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  169. Tomkins, S. S. (1970). Affect as he primary motivational system. In M. B. Arnold (Ed.), Feelings and emotions: The Loyola symposium (pp. 101-110). New York, NY: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  170. Tong, E. M. (2010). The sufficiency and necessity of appraisals for negative emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 24, 692–701.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  171. Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2008). The evolutionary psychology of the emotions and their relationship to internal regulatory variables. In M. Lewis, J. Haviland, & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd ed., pp. 114–137). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  172. Valentino, N. A., Gregorowicz, K., & Groenendyk, E. W. (2009). Efficacy, emotions and the habit of participation. Political Behavior, 31, 307–330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  173. Van Stekelenburg, J., & Klandermans, B. (2013). The social psychology of protest. Current Sociology, 61, 886–905.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  174. van Zomeren, M., Spears, R., & Leach, C. W. (2008). Exploring psychological mechanisms of collective action: Does relevance of group identity influence how people cope with collective disadvantage? British Journal of Social Psychology, 47, 353–372.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  175. Wagner, M. (2014). Fear and anger in great Britain: Blame assignment and emotional reactions to the financial crisis. Political Behavior, 36, 683–703.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  176. Waters, E. (2002). Comments on Strange Situation classification. Retrieved from http://www.psychology.sunysb.edu/attachment/measures/content/ss_scoring.pdf.
  177. Webster, S. W. (in press). Anger and declining trust in government in the American electorate. Political Behaviorhttps://doi.org/10.1007/s11109-017-9431-7.
  178. White, S. F., Brislin, S. J., Sinclair, S., & Blair, J. R. (2014). Punishing unfairness: Rewarding or the organization of a reactively aggressive response? Human Brain Mapping, 35(5), 2137–2147.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  179. Williams, J. E. (2010). Anger/hostility and cardiovascular disease. In M. Potegal, G. Stemmler, & C. Spielberger (Eds.), International handbook of anger (pp. 435–447). New York, NY: Springer.Google Scholar
  180. Wortman, C. B., & Brehm, J. W. (1975). Responses to uncontrollable outcomes: An integration of reactance theory and the learned helplessness model. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 8, pp. 277–336). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  181. Zernike, K. (2010). Boiling mad: Inside Tea Party America. New York, NY: Times Books.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of PsychologyRutgers UniversityCamdenUSA

Personalised recommendations