Advertisement

Thinking Through Time: From Collective Memories to Collective Futures

  • Constance de Saint-Laurent
Chapter
Part of the Palgrave Studies in Creativity and Culture book series (PASCC)

Abstract

This chapter explores the links between collective memory and the imagination of collective futures. Drawing on works on imagination and autobiographical memory, it first discusses the role of past experiences in imagining the future. It then highlights the consequences of this perspective for collective memories and collective futures, arguing that the former provides the basis for the latter. Three case studies are presented, each illustrating a different type of relation between collective memory and imagination. This will lead me to the conclusion that representations of the world are characterised by “temporal heteroglossia”, the simultaneous presence of multiple periods of time, and that they mediate the relation between collective memory and imagination, allowing us to “think through time”.

Keywords

Future imagination Collective memory Metamemory Heteroglossia Analogies Uses of the past 

References

  1. Asimov, I. (1978). Foreword. In R. Holdstock (Ed.), Encyclopedia of science fiction (pp. 6–8). London: Octopus Books.Google Scholar
  2. Bakhtin, M. M. (1981). The dialogic imagination: Four essays. Austin, TX: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
  3. Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering. A study in experimental and social psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  4. Berntsen, D., & Bohn, A. (2010). Remembering and forecasting: The relation between autobiographical memory and episodic future thinking. Memory & Cognition, 38(3), 265–278.  https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.38.3.265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Brescó de Luna, I. (2018, this volume). Imagining collective futures in time: Prolepsis and the regimes of historicity. In C. de Saint-Laurent, S. Obradović, & K. R. Carriere (Eds.), Imagining collective futures: Perspectives from social, cultural, and political psychology (pp. 109–128). London: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
  6. Brescó de Luna, I. (2017). The end into the beginning. Prolepsis and the reconstruction of the collective past. Culture & Psychology, 23(2), 280–294.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1354067X17695761.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Brockmeier, J. (2010). After the archive. Remapping memory. Culture & Psychology, 16(1), 5–35.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1354067X09353212 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Cole, M. (2007). Phylogeny and cultural history in ontogeny. Journal of Physiology-Paris, 101, 236–246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. D’Argembeau, A. (2012). Autobiographical memory and future thinking. In D. Bernsten & C. Rubin (Eds.), Understanding autobiographical memory (pp. 311–330). New York: Cambridge University Press. Retrieved from  https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139021937.022.
  10. de Saint-Laurent, C. (2012). Dialogue in democratic institutions. A dialogical analysis of the French parliamentary debates on immigration of 2006 (Unpublished dissertation). London: London School of Economics.Google Scholar
  11. de Saint-Laurent, C. (2014). “I would rather be hanged than agree with you!”. Collective memory and the definition of the nation in parliamentary debates on immigration. Outlines. Critical Practice Studies, 15(3), 22–53.Google Scholar
  12. de Saint-Laurent, C. (2017a). Staying on topic: Doing research between improvisation and systematisation. In C. Wegener, N. Meier, & E. Maslo (Eds.), Cultivating creativity in methodology and research. In praise of detours (pp. 143–152). London: Palgrave.Google Scholar
  13. de Saint-Laurent, C. (2017b). Personal trajectories, collective memories. Remembering and the life-course. Culture & Psychology, 23(2), 263–279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. de Saint-Laurent, C. (2017c). Trajectories of resistance and historical reflections. In N. Chaudhary, G. Marsico, P. Hviid, & J. Villadsen (Eds.), Resistance in everyday life: Constructing cultural experiences. Singapore: Springer.Google Scholar
  15. de Saint-Laurent, C. (2018a). Memory acts. A theory for the study of collective memory in everyday life. Journal of Constructivist Psychology, 31(2), 148–162.  https://doi.org/10.1080/10720537.2016.1271375.
  16. de Saint-Laurent, C. (2018b). Beyond collective memory. A sociocultural perspective on historical representations. Unpublished PhD Thesis. Université de Neuchâtel, Neuchâtel.Google Scholar
  17. de Saint-Laurent, C. (In preparation). Social thinking and history. A sociocultural perspective on how we represent the past. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  18. de Saint-Laurent, C., Brescó de Luna, I., Awad, S. H., & Wagoner, B. (2017). Collective memory and social sciences in the post-truth era. Culture & Psychology, 23(2), 147–155.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1354067X17695769.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. de Saint-Laurent, C., & Glăveanu, V. P. (in press). Cultural psychology and politics. Otherness, democracy and the refugee crisis. In B. Wagoner, I. Brescó de Luna, & V. P. Glăveanu (Eds.), The road to actuliazed democracy. Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
  20. de Saint-Laurent, C., & Zittoun, T. (2017). Memory in life transitions. In B. Wagoner (Ed.), Handbook of memory and culture (pp. 209–236). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  21. Dudai, Y., & Carruthers, M. (2005). The Janus face of Mnemosyne. Nature, 434(7033), 567–567.  https://doi.org/10.1038/434567a.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Foucault, M. (1993). Surveiller et punir. Naissance de la prison. Paris: Gallimard.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Gergen, K. J. (1973). Social psychology as history. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 26, 309–320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Gillespie, A. (2008). Social representations, alternative representations and semantic barriers. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 38, 375–391.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-5914.2008.00376.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Glăveanu, V. P., & de Saint-Laurent, C. (2015). Political imagination, otherness and the European crisis. Europe’s Journal of Psychology, 11(4), 557–564.  https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v11i4.1085.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  26. Grossen, M. (2010). Interaction analysis and psychology. A dialogical perspective. Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 44(1), 1–22.  https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-009-9108-9.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Habermas, T. (2012). Identity, emotion, and the social matrix of autobiographical memory. A psychoanalytic narrative view. In D. Bernsten & D. Rubin (Eds.), Understanding autobiographical memory. Theories and approaches (pp. 33–53). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Holyoak, K. J. (2005). Analogy. In K. J. Holyoak & R. G. Morrison (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of thinking and reasoning (pp. 117–142). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  29. Liu, J. H., & Hilton, D. (2005). How the past weighs on the present. Social representations of history and their role in identity politics. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 537–556.  https://doi.org/10.1348/014466605X27162.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Marková, I., Linell, P., Grossen, M., & Salazar Orvig, A. (2007). Dialogue in focus groups. Exploring socially shared knowledge. London: Equinox.Google Scholar
  31. Mattli, F., Schnitzspahn, K. M., Studerus-Germann, A., Brehmer, Y., & Zöllig, J. (2014). Prospective memory across the lifespan. Investigating the contribution of retrospective and prospective processes. Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. Section B, Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 21(5), 515–543.  https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2013.837860.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Merck, C., Topcu, M. N., & Hirst, W. (2016). Collective mental time travel. Creating a shared future through our shared past. Memory Studies, 9(3), 284–294.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1750698016645236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Mullally, S. L., & Maguire, E. A. (2013). Memory, imagination, and predicting the future. A common brain mechanism? The Neuroscientist, 20(3), 220–234.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858413495091.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Obradović, S. (2018, this volume). Creating integration: A case study from Serbia and the EU. In C. de Saint-Laurent, S. Obradović, & K. R. Carriere (Eds.), Imagining collective futures: Perspectives from social, cultural, and political psychology (pp. 237–254). London: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
  35. Schacter, D. L., & Addis, D. R. (2007). Constructive memory. The ghosts of past and future. Nature, 445(7123), 27–27.  https://doi.org/10.1038/445027a.CrossRefPubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  36. Spellman, B. A., & Holyoak, K. J. (1992). If Saddam is Hitler then who is George Bush? Analogical mapping between systems of social roles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62(6), 913–933.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.62.6.913.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Szpunar, P. M., & Szpunar, K. K. (2016). Collective future thought. Concept, function, and implications for collective memory studies. Memory Studies, 9(4), 376–389.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1750698015615660.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic memory. From mind to brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 1–25.  https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135114.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Vygotsky, L. S. (1991). Imagination and creativity of the adolescent. Soviet Psychology, 21(1), 73–88.Google Scholar
  40. Vygotsky, L. S. (2004). Imagination and creativity in childhood. Journal of Russian and East European Psychology, 42(1), 7–97.Google Scholar
  41. Wagoner, B. (2017). The constructive mind. Bartlett’s psychology in reconstruction. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Wertsch, J. (2008). The narrative organization of collective memory. Ethos, 36, 120–135.  https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1548-1352.2008.00007.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Zittoun, T. (2006). Transitions. Development through symbolic resources. Greenwich: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
  44. Zittoun, T., & de Saint-Laurent, C. (2015). Life-creativity. Imagining one’s life. In V. P. Glăveanu, A. Gillespie, & J. Valsiner (Eds.), Rethinking creativity. Contributions from cultural psychology (pp. 58–75). Hove/New York: Routeldge.Google Scholar
  45. Zittoun, T., Duveen, G., Gillespie, A., Ivinson, G., & Psaltis, C. (2003). The use of symbolic resources in developmental transitions. Culture & Psychology, 9, 415–448.  https://doi.org/10.1177/1354067X0394006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Zittoun, T., & Gillespie, A. (2018, this volume). Imagining the collective future: A sociocultural perspective. In C. de Saint-Laurent, S. Obradović, & K. R. Carriere (Eds.), Imagining collective futures: Perspectives from social, cultural, and political psychology (pp. 15–37). London: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
  47. Zittoun, T., & Gillespie, A. (2015). Integrating experiences. Body and mind moving between contexts. In B. Wagoner, N. Chaudhary, & P. Hviid (Eds.), Integrating experiences. Body and mind moving between contexts (pp. 3–49). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
  48. Zittoun, T., & Gillespie, A. (2016). Imagination in human and cultural development. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
  49. Zittoun, T., Valsiner, J., Vedeler, D., Salgado, J., Gonçalves, M. M., & Ferring, D. (2013). Human development in the life course. Melodies of living. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© The Author(s) 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Constance de Saint-Laurent
    • 1
  1. 1.University of NeuchâtelNeuchâtelSwitzerland

Personalised recommendations