Advertisement

Neurophilosophy and Neuroethics: Template for Neuropsychoanalysis?

  • Georg Northoff
Chapter

Abstract

The encounter of neuroscience has led to intense debate in philosophy on how its originally mind-based concepts are related to the brain and its neural function. While some proponents in especially the Anglo-American world suggest reductive replacement of philosophy by neuroscience as manifest in what they call neurophilosophy, the opponents claim for a more non-reductive form of neurophilosophy where both philosophy and neuroscience are closely intertwined but distinct. I here sketch the field of such non-reductive neurophilosophy by distinguishing different domains, empirical (neuroscientific investigation of originally philosophical concepts) theoretical (methodological and conceptual issues), and practical (neuroethical questions) neurophilosophy. In conclusion, a non-reductive neurophilosophy opens the door for a truly transdisciplinary exchange between philosophy and neuroscience which will lead to novel questions and approaches in both disciplines. This, in turn, will also make possible intensive dialogue between psychoanalysis and neuroscience, i.e. neuropsychoanalysis.

Keywords

Neurophilosophy Non-reductive Empirical Theoretical Neuroethics 

References

  1. Appelbaum PS, Grisso T, Frank E, O’Donnell S, Kupfer DJ. Competence of depressed patients for consent to research. Am J Psychiatry. 1999;156(9):1380–4.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Baars BJ. Global workspace theory of consciousness: toward a cognitive neuroscience of human experience. Prog Brain Res. 2005;150:45–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Baars BJ, Franklin S. An architectural model of conscious and unconscious brain functions: Global Workspace Theory and IDA. Neural Netw. 2007;20(9):955–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Baars BJ, Ramsoy TZ, Laureys S. Brain, conscious experience and the observing self. Trends Neurosci. 2003;26(12):671–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bechtel W, Mandik P, Mundale J. Philosophy meets the neurosciences. In: Bechtel W, Mandik P, Mundale J, Stufflebeam RS, editors. Philosophy and the neurosciences: a reader. Oxford: Blackwell; 2001.Google Scholar
  6. Bennett MR, Hacker PMS. Philosophical foundations of neuroscience. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing; 2003.Google Scholar
  7. Bickle J, Mandik P, Landreth A. The philosophy of neuroscience. Journal [serial on the Internet]. 2006. Available from: http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/neuroscience/.
  8. Bowers D, Blonder L, Heilman K. The Florida affect battery. Center of Neuropsychological Studies; 1992.Google Scholar
  9. Breden TM, Vollmann J. The cognitive based approach of capacity assessment in psychiatry: a philosophical critique of the MacCAT-T. Health Care Anal. 2004;12(4):273–83. discussion 65-72CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Breidbach O. Die Materialisierung des Ichs - Eine Geschichte der Hirnforschung im 19. und 20. Jahrhundert. Frankfurt: Suhrkamp; 1997.Google Scholar
  11. Buckner RL, Andrews-Hanna JR, Schacter DL. The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1124:1–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Candilis PJ, Geppert CM, Fletcher KE, Lidz CW, Appelbaum PS. Willingness of subjects with thought disorder to participate in research. Schizophr Bull. 2006;32(1):159–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Candilis PJ, Fletcher KE, Geppert CM, Lidz CW, Appelbaum PS. A direct comparison of research decision-making capacity: schizophrenia/schizoaffective, medically ill, and non-ill subjects. Schizophr Res. 2008;99(1-3):350–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Carpenter Jr WT, Gold JM, Lahti AC, Queern CA, Conley RR, Bartko JJ, Kovnick J, Appelbaum PS. Decisional capacity for informed consent in schizophrenia research. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57(6):533–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Charland LC. Appreciation and emotion: theoretical reflections on the MacArthur Treatment Competence Study. Kennedy Inst Ethics J. 1998;8(4):359–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Churchland P. Neurophilosophy: toward a unified science of the mind-brain. Cambridge: MIT Press; 1986.Google Scholar
  17. Churchland PS. Self-representation in nervous systems. Science. 2002;296(5566):308–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Dalgleish T. The emotional brain. Nat Rev. 2004;5(7):583–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Damasio AR. How the brain creates the mind. Sci Am. 1999;281(6):112–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Damasio A. Feelings of emotion and the self. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2003a;1001:253–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Damasio A. Mental self: the person within. Nature. 2003b;423(6937):227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Damoiseaux JS, Rombouts SA, Barkhof F, Scheltens P, Stam CJ, Smith SM, Beckmann CF. Consistent resting-state networks across healthy subjects. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2006;103(37):13848–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Decety J, Lamm C. Human empathy through the lens of social neuroscience. Sci World J. 2006;6:1146–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Dziobek I, Rogers K, Fleck S, Bahnemann M, Heekeren HR, Wolf OT, Convit A. Dissociation of cognitive and emotional empathy in adults with Asperger syndrome using the Multifaceted Empathy Test (MET). J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38(3):464–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Edelman GM. Naturalizing consciousness: a theoretical framework. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003;100(9):5520–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Fan Y, Duncan NW, de Greck M, Northoff G. Is there a core neural network in empathy? An fMRI based quantitative meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35(3):903–11.  https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.10.009.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  27. Fox MD, Raichle ME. Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nat Rev. 2007;8(9):700–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Fransson P. Spontaneous low-frequency BOLD signal fluctuations: an fMRI investigation of the resting-state default mode of brain function hypothesis. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005;26(1):15–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Gallagher S. Philosophical conception of the self: implications for cognitive science. Trends Cogn Sci. 2000;4:14–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Gallagher HL, Frith CD. Functional imaging of theory of mind. Trends Cogn Sci. 2003;7(2):77–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Grisso T, Appelbaum PS, Hill-Fotouhi C. The MacCAT-T: a clinical tool to assess patients’ capacities to make treatment decisions. Psychiatr Serv. 1997;48(11):1415–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Hein G, Singer T. I feel how you feel but not always: the empathic brain and its modulation. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2008;18(2):153–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Holstege G, Bandler R, Saper CB. The emotional motor system. Prog Brain Res. 1996;107:3–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Howe V, Foister K, Jenkins K, Skene L, Copolov D, Keks N. Competence to give informed consent in acute psychosis is associated with symptoms rather than diagnosis. Schizophr Res. 2005;77(2-3):211–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Jeste DV, Depp CA, Palmer BW. Magnitude of impairment in decisional capacity in people with schizophrenia compared to normal subjects: an overview. Schizophr Bull. 2006;32(1):121–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Keenan JP, Wheeler M, Platek SM, Lardi G, Lassonde M. Self-face processing in a callosotomy patient. Eur J Neurosci. 2003;18(8):2391–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Kelley WM, Macrae CN, Wyland CL, Caglar S, Inati S, Heatherton TF. Finding the self? An event-related fMRI study. J Cogn Neurosci. 2002;14(5):785–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Kircher T, David A. The self in neuroscience and psychiatry. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2003.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Koren D, Poyurovsky M, Seidman LJ, Goldsmith M, Wenger S, Klein EM. The neuropsychological basis of competence to consent in first-episode schizophrenia: a pilot metacognitive study. Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(6):609–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Lambie JA, Marcel AJ. Consciousness and the varieties of emotion experience: a theoretical framework. Psychol Rev. 2002;109(2):219–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. LeDoux J. Synaptic self: how our brains become who we are. New York: Viking; 2002.Google Scholar
  42. Legrand D, Ruby P. What is self-specific? Theoretical investigation and critical review of neuroimaging results. Psychol Rev. 2009;116(1):252–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Marcel AJ, Lambie JA. How many selves in emotion experience? Reply to Dalgleish and power. Psychol Rev. 2004;111(3):820–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Metzinger T, Gallese V. The emergence of a shared action ontology: building blocks for a theory. Conscious Cogn. 2003;12(4):549–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Moreno JD. Neuroethics: an agenda for neuroscience and society. Nat Rev. 2003;4(2):149–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Moser DJ, Schultz SK, Arndt S, Benjamin ML, Fleming FW, Brems CS, Paulsen JS, Appelbaum PS, Andreasen NC. Capacity to provide informed consent for participation in schizophrenia and HIV research. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159(7):1201–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Noe A, Thompson E. Are there neural correlates of consciousness? J Conscious Stud. 2004;11:3–28.Google Scholar
  48. Northoff G. Personale Identität und operative Eingriffe in das Gehirn. Paderborn: Mentis; 2001.Google Scholar
  49. Northoff G. Philosophy of the brain. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing; 2004.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Northoff G. Neuroscience of decision making and informed consent: an investigation in neuroethics. J Med Ethics. 2006;32(2):70–3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Northoff G, Bermpohl F. Cortical midline structures and the self. Trends Cogn Sci. 2004;8(3):102–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Northoff G, Panksepp J. The trans-species concept of self and the subcortical-cortical midline system. Trends Cogn Sci. 2008;12(7):259–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Northoff G, Heinzel A, de Greck M, Bermpohl F, Dobrowolny H, Panksepp J. Self-referential processing in our brain–a meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. NeuroImage. 2006;31(1):440–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Northoff G, Walter M, Schulte RF, Beck J, Dydak U, Henning A, Boeker H, Grimm S, Boesiger P. GABA concentrations in the human anterior cingulate cortex predict negative BOLD responses in fMRI. Nat Neurosci. 2007;10(12):1515–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Palmer BW, Jeste DV. Relationship of individual cognitive abilities to specific components of decisional capacity among middle-aged and older patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2006;32(1):98–106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Palmer BW, Dunn LB, Appelbaum PS, Jeste DV. Correlates of treatment-related decision-making capacity among middle-aged and older patients with schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61(3):230–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  57. Panksepp J. Affective neuroscience: the foundations of human and animal emotions. New York: Oxford University Press; 1998a.Google Scholar
  58. Panksepp J. The preconscious substrates of consciousness: affective states and the evolutionary origins of the self. J Conscious Stud. 1998b;5:566–82.Google Scholar
  59. Panksepp J. At the interface of the affective, behavioral, and cognitive neurosciences: decoding the emotional feelings of the brain. Brain Cogn. 2003;52(1):4–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Panksepp J, Northoff G. The trans-species core SELF: the emergence of active cultural and neuro-ecological agents through self-related processing within subcortical-cortical midline networks. Conscious Cogn. 2008;18(1):193–215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Phan KL, Taylor SF, Welsh RC, Ho SH, Britton JC, Liberzon I. Neural correlates of individual ratings of emotional salience: a trial-related fMRI study. NeuroImage. 2004;21(2):768–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Popper K, Eccles J. Das Ich und sein Gehirn. München: Piper; 1989.Google Scholar
  63. Quine WVO. Epistemology naturalized. In: Quine WVO, editor. Ontological relativity and other essays. New York: Columbia University Press; 1969.Google Scholar
  64. Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA, Shulman GL. A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2001;98(2):676–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  65. Rilling JK, Barks SK, Parr LA, Preuss TM, Faber TL, Pagnoni G, Bremner JD, Votaw JR. A comparison of resting-state brain activity in humans and chimpanzees. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104(43):17146–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Roberts LW, Warner TD, Brody JL, Roberts B, Lauriello J, Lyketsos C. Patient and psychiatrist ratings of hypothetical schizophrenia research protocols: assessment of harm potential and factors influencing participation decisions. Am J Psychiatry. 2002;159(4):573–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Roskies A. Neuroethics for the new millenium. Neuron. 2002;35(1):21–3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Searle JR. The future of philosophy. Philos Trans R Soc Lond. 1999;354(1392):2069–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Searle J. Mind: a brief introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2004.Google Scholar
  70. Singer T, Seymour B, O'Doherty JP, Stephan KE, Dolan RJ, Frith CD. Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others. Nature. 2006;439(7075):466–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Stroup S, Appelbaum P, Swartz M, Patel M, Davis S, Jeste D, Kim S, Keefe R, Manschreck T, McEvoy J, Lieberman J. Decision-making capacity for research participation among individuals in the CATIE schizophrenia trial. Schizophr Res. 2005;80(1):1–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Stuss DT, Gallup GG Jr, Alexander MP. The frontal lobes are necessary for theory of mind. Brain. 2001;124(Pt 2):279–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Turk DJ, Heatherton TF, Kelley WM, Funnell MG, Gazzaniga MS, Macrae CN. Mike or me? Self-recognition in a split-brain patient. Nat Neurosci. 2002;5(9):841–2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  74. Uddin LQ, Iacoboni M, Lange C, Keenan JP. The self and social cognition: the role of cortical midline structures and mirror neurons. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11(4):153–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. de Vignemont F, Singer T. The empathic brain: how, when and why? Trends Cogn Sci. 2006;10(10):435–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Vincent JL, Patel GH, Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Baker JT, Van Essen DC, Zempel JM, Snyder LH, Corbetta M, Raichle ME. Intrinsic functional architecture in the anaesthetized monkey brain. Nature. 2007;447(7140):83–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  77. Vogeley K, Fink GR. Neural correlates of the first-person-perspective. Trends Cogn Sci. 2003;7(1):38–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Walter H. Neurophilosophie der Willensfreiheit: von libertarischen Illusionen zum Konzept natürlicher Autonomie. Paderborn: Mentis; 1998.Google Scholar
  79. Wicker B, Ruby P, Royet JP, Fonlupt P. A relation between rest and the self in the brain? Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 2003;43(2):224–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Mind, Brain Imaging, and Neuroethics, Institute of Mental Health ResearchUniversity of OttawaOttawaCanada

Personalised recommendations