A Semiotic Methodology for Animal Studies pp 55-83 | Cite as
Necessary and Problematic Definitions
Chapter
First Online:
Abstract
Conception sub-question n°2: what definitions should we all agree upon from an interdisciplinary standpoint?
First of all, this chapter will introduce the definitions considered necessary for a semiotic work about animals. The suggested definitions will revisit the concepts of emotion (in its general sense), consciousness and memory. Later on, it will present the cases deemed problematic regarding definitions: the concepts of language, emotions (in a more specific sense: fear, grief, anger etc), intelligence and culture. Solutions will be submitted each time to avoid the issue of definitions.
Keywords
Animal studies Definitions Lexicology Language Culture Emotions Intelligence MemoryReferences
- Académie Nationale de Médecine – France. (2011). Bulletin de l’Académie Nationale de Médecine, 195(8), 1927–1944.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Armony, J., & Vuilleumier, P. (Eds.). (2013). The Cambridge handbook of human affective neuroscience. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
- Bekoff, M. (2007). The emotional lives of animals. Novato: New World Library.Google Scholar
- Bekoff, M. (2009). Les émotions des animaux. Paris: Éditions Payot & Rivages.Google Scholar
- Benveniste, E. (1966). Problèmes de linguistique générale. Paris: Gallimard.Google Scholar
- Ben-ze’ev, A. (2010). The thing called emotion. In P. Goldie (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of philosophy of emotion (p. 61). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
- Bickle, J. (Ed.). (2009). The Oxford handbook of philosophy and neuroscience. Oxford: Oxford university press.Google Scholar
- Brennan, B., Malakoff, D., & Visty, R. (1999). A conversation with Koko. Nature PBS, août 1999.Google Scholar
- Brown, K. (Ed.). (2006). Encyclopedia of language and linguistics (Vol. 1). San Diego: Elsevier Distributor.Google Scholar
- Chapouthier, G. (2009). Kant et le chimpanzé, essai sur l’être humain, la morale et l’art. Paris: Belin.Google Scholar
- Chapouthier, G., Kreutzer, M., & Menini, C. (Eds.). (1980). Psychophysiologie. Le système nerveux et le comportement. Ligugé: Études vivantes.Google Scholar
- Chevalier-Skolnikoff, S. (Ed.). (1977). Primate bio-social development: Biological, social and ecological determinants. New York: Garland.Google Scholar
- Christen, Y. (2011). L’Animal est-il une personne ? Barcelone: Flammarion.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- de Saussure, F. (2002). Ecrits de linguistique générale. Paris: Gallimard.Google Scholar
- De Waal, F. (1997). Good natured: Origins of right and wrong in humans and other animals. Harvard: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
- De Waal, F., & Robert, M. (2002). De la réconciliation chez les primates. Paris: Flammarion.Google Scholar
- De Waal, F., Uno, H., et al. (1996). Behavioral retardation in a macaque with autosomal trisomy and aging mother. American Journal of Mental Retardation, 100(4), 378–390.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Dorigo, M., & Stützle, T. (2004). Ant colony optimization. Cambridge: MIT Press/Bradford Books.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Eco, U. (1979). A theory of semiotics. Bloomington: Indiana university press.Google Scholar
- Eco, U. (1999). Kant et l’ornithorynque. Paris: Grasset.Google Scholar
- Fedigan, L., & Fedigan, L. M. (1977). The social development of a handicaped infant in a free-living troop of Japanese monkeys. In S. Chevalier-Skolnikoff (Ed.), Primate bio-social development : Biological, social an ecological determinants (pp. 205–222). New York: Garland.Google Scholar
- Fouts, R., & Mills, S. T. (1998). L’école des chimpanzés: ce que les chimpanzés nous apprennent sur l’humanité. Paris: J. C. Lattès.Google Scholar
- Goldie, P. (Ed.). (2010). The Oxford handbook of philosophy of emotion. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
- Goodall, J. (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe. Cambridge: The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
- Greimas, A. (1988). De la nostalgie : étude de sémantique lexicale. In Hommage à Bernard Pottier vol. 1 (p. 343). Paris: Klincksieck.Google Scholar
- Greimas, A., & Fontanille, J. (1991). La Sémiotique des passions. Des états de choses aux états d’âme. Paris: Seuil.Google Scholar
- Gucwa, G., & Ehmann, J. (1985). To whom it may concern: An investigation of the art of elephants. New York: W. W. Norton & Co.Google Scholar
- Jakobson, R. (2003). Essais de linguistique générale, 1: Les fondations du langage. Paris: Éditions de Minuit.Google Scholar
- Konishi, M. (1965). The role of auditory feedback in the control of vocalization in the white-crowned sparrow. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 22(7), 770–783.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Krief, S., Hladik, C. M., & Haxaire, C. (2005). Ethnomedicinal and bioactive properties of plants ingested by wild chimpanzees in Uganda. Journal of Ethnopharmacology, 101, 1–15.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Langergraber, K. E., Watts, D. P., Vigilant, L., & Mitani, J. C. (2017). Group augmentation, collective action, and territorial boundary patrols by male chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114(28), 7337–7342.PubMedPubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Maestripieri, D. (Ed.). (2003). Primate psychology. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
- Manser, M., Bell, M., & Fletcher, L. (2001). The information that receivers extract from alarm calls in suricates. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 268(1484), 2485–2491.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Markowitz, H. (1982). Behavioral enrichment in the zoo. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.Google Scholar
- Marler, P., & Mundinger, P. (1971). Vocal learning in birds. In H. Moltz (Ed.), Ontogeny of vertebrate behaviour (pp. 389–450). New York: Academic.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Matsuzawa, T. (1997). The death of an infant chimpanzee at Bossou, Guinea. Pan Africa News, 4(1), 4–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Moltz, H. (Ed.). (1971). Ontogeny of vertebrate behaviour. New-York. Academic.Google Scholar
- Morris, D. (1962). Biologie de l’art. Paris: Stock.Google Scholar
- Moss, C. (1989). La Longue Marche des éléphants. Paris: Robert Laffont.Google Scholar
- Mounin, G. (Ed.). (2004). Dictionnaire de la linguistique. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.Google Scholar
- Naguib, M. (2006). Animal communication: Overview. In K. Brown (Ed.), Encyclopedia of language and linguistics (Vol. 1, p. 277). San Diego: Elsevier Distributor.Google Scholar
- Nishida, T., et al. (2010). Chimpanzee behaviour in the wild: An audio-visual encyclopedia. Tokyo/New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Parr, L. A., & Maestripieri, D. (2003). Nonvocal communication. In D. Maestripieri (Ed.), Primate psychology (pp. 324–258). Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
- Pepperberg, I. (1999). Alex studies. Cognitive and communicative abilities of grey parrot. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
- Porter, J. W. (1977). Pseudorca stranding. Oceans, 10, 8–15.Google Scholar
- Prinz, J. (2010). The moral emotions. In P. Goldie (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of philosophy of emotion (pp. 519–538). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
- Pugmire, D. (2010). Saying it. In P. Goldie (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of philosophy of emotion (p. 384). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
- Rose, N. A. (2000). A death in the family. In M. Bekoff (Ed.), The smile of the dolphin. Remarkable accounts of animal emotions (p. 144). New York: Discovery Books.Google Scholar
- Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1989). La pertinence: Communication et cognition. Paris: les Éditions de Minuit.Google Scholar
- Stoeger, A. S., Mietchen, D., Oh, S., de Silva, S., Herbst, C. T., Kwon, S., & Fitch, T. (2012). An asian elephant imitates human speech. Current Biology, 22, 2144–2148.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Sufka, K., Weldon, M., & Allen, C. (2009). The case for animal emotions: Modeling neuropsychiatric disorders. In J. Bickle (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of philosophy and neuroscience (pp. 522–536). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
- Teleki, G. (1973). Group response to the accidental death of a chimpanzee in Gombe National Park. Folia Primatologica, 20(2), 81–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Thoen, H., How, M. J., Chiou, T. H., & Marshall, J. (2014). A different form of color vision in mantis shrimp. Science, 343(6169), 411–413.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Watanabe, S., & Kuczaj, S. (2013). Emotions of animals and humans – Comparative perspectives. Tokyo: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Wechkin, S., Masserman, J. H., & Terris, W. (1964). Shock to a conspecific as an aversive stimulus. Psychonomic Science, 1, 47–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Whiten, A., Horner, V., & De Waal, F. (2005). Conformity to cultural norms of tool use in chimpanzees. Nature, 437(7059), 737–740.PubMedCrossRefPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
- Wittgenstein, L. (1961). Investigations philosophiques. In Tractatus logico-philosophicus suivi de Investigations philosophiques (p. 1961). Paris: Gallimard.Google Scholar
- Zahn, R., de Oliveira-Souza, R., & Moll, J. (2013). Moral emotions. In J. Armony & P. Vuilleumier (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of human affective neuroscience (pp. 491–508). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Zuberbühler, K. (2013). Referential signalling in non-human primates: Cognitive precursors and limitations for the evolution of language. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 33, 265–307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Copyright information
© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019