Advertisement

The Structure of Scientific Fraud: The Relationship Between Paradigms and Misconduct

  • Ben Trubody
Chapter
Part of the Synthese Library book series (SYLI, volume 413)

Abstract

This chapter argues that the level of difficulty in committing scientific fraud is a route to analysing the similarities and differences between the sciences. In The Structure of Scientific Revolutions Kuhn presents a paradigmatic theory of scientific change. “Paradigms”, I argue, set a limit for things that pretend to be scientific, where the possibility and potential for “fraud” is dependent upon the strength or weakness of the paradigm. For the dishonest scientist two types of expertise become useful: (a) “contributional expertise” and (b) “interactional expertise”. The weaker the paradigm is, the more these two types of expertise begin to blur; what constitutes an act of contribution becomes unclear. I highlight a criterion of “reasonableness” and “significance” as being key to “contribution”. A claim that is unreasonable or trivial will either not be taken seriously, or draw too much critical attention, both unsuitable for fraud. What is deemed “reasonable” or “significant”, however, is relative to the field of practice: in some sciences, different practitioners are allowed to hold fundamentally contradictory positions, whereas in others rejection of a widely accepted belief would be taken to signify poor practice.

Keywords

Anaesthesiology Cold fusion Contributional expertise Interactional expertise Kuhnian paradigm Scientific fraud Scientific misconduct Semi-conductor Social psychology 

References

  1. Aarts, A. A., Anderson, J. E., Anderson, C. J., et al. (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science, 349, aac4716–aac4718.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Baker, M. (2016). 1,500 scientists lift the lid on reproducibility. Nature, 533, 452–454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Beasly, M. R., Datta, S., Kogelnik, H., Kroemer, H., & Monroe, D. (2002). Report of the investigation committee on the possibility of scientific misconduct in the work of Hendrik Schon and coauthors, APS. http://publish.aps.org/reports/lucentrep.pdf. Accessed 15 Apr 2016.
  4. Bird, A. (2012). What can cognitive science tell us about scientific revolutions? Theoria, 27, 293–321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Borrell, B. (2009). A medical Madoff: Anesthesiologist faked data in 21 studies. Scientific American http://www.scientificamerican.com/article/a-medical-madoff-anesthestesiologist-faked-data. Accessed 6 Feb 2016.
  6. Carlisle, J. (2016). Post operative nausea and vomiting research: Methodology, assessment and strength of research. In T. J. Gan & A. S. Habib (Eds.), Postoperative nausea and vomiting: A practical guide (pp. 156–169). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Cedarbaum, D. G. (1983). Paradigms. Studies in the History & Philosophy of Science, 14, 173–213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Chang, H. J. (2014). Economics: The user’s guide. London: Pelican.Google Scholar
  9. Collins, H., & Evans, R. (2007). Rethinking expertise. Chicago: Chicago University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Colquhoun, D. (2014). An investigation of the false discovery rate and the misinterpretation of p-values. Royal Society Open Science, 1(3), 140216.  https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.140216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Cyranoski, D. (2012). Retraction record rocks community. Nature, 489(7416). http://www.nature.com/news/retraction-record-rocks-community-1.11434. Accessed 17 Feb 2016.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Decullier, E., Huot, L., Samson, G., & Maisonneuve, H. (2013). Visibility of retractions: A cross-sectional one-year study. BMC Research Notes, 6, 238.  https://doi.org/10.1186/1756-0500-6-238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Dreyfus, H. (1980). Holism and hermeneutics. The Review of Metaphysics, 34, 3–23.Google Scholar
  14. Feynman, R. (2001). Cargo cult science: Some remarks on science, pseudoscience, and learning how to not fool yourself. In J. Robbins (Ed.), The pleasure of finding things out: The best of the short works of Richard P. Feynman (pp. 205–216). London: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
  15. Fraser, D. A. S. (2014). Why does statistics have two theories? In X. Lin, C. Genest, D. Banks, et al. (Eds.), Past, present, and future of statistical science (pp. 237–254). Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Gattei, S. (2008). Thomas Kuhn’s “linguistic turn” and the legacy of logical empiricism: Incommensurability, rationality and the search for truth. Aldershot, UK: Ashgate Publishing Limited.Google Scholar
  17. Glasser, S. P., & Duval, S. (2014). Meta-analysis, evidence-based medicine, and clinical guidelines. In S. P. Glasser (Ed.), Essentials of clinical research (pp. 203–223). London: Springer.Google Scholar
  18. Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2005). Why most published research findings are false. PLoS Medicine, 2(8), e124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. John, L. K., Loewenstein, G., & Prelec, D. (2012). Measuring the prevalence of questionable research practices with incentives for truth-telling. Psychological Science, 23, 524–532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Jolley, D. (2012). Fujii, anesthesia & research fraud. Gas Exchange. http://gasexchange.com/articles/fujii-anesthesia-research-fraud/. Accessed 26 June 2016.
  21. Kaiser, D. (2009). Physics and pixie dust. American Scientist, 97, 469–478.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Kuhn, T. S. (1993). Afterwords. In P. Horwich (Ed.), World changes: Thomas Kuhn and the nature of science (pp. 311–341). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  23. Kuhn, T. S. (1996). The structure of scientific revolutions (3rd ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Laudan, L. (1977). Progress and its problems: Towards a theory of scientific growth. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.Google Scholar
  25. Lewan, M. (2017). Here’s the settlement – Getting the license back was Rossi’s top priority. An Impossible Invention, https://animpossibleinvention.com/2017/07/18/heres-the-settlement-getting-the-license-back-was-rossis-top-priority/. Accessed 22 July 2017.
  26. Lewin, R. (1997). The bone of contention: Controversies in the search for human origins. Chicago: University Press.Google Scholar
  27. Markowitz, D. M., & Hancock, J. T. (2014). Linguistic traces of a scientific fraud: The case of Diederik Stapel. PLoS One, 9(8): e105937.  https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0105937. Accessed 18 Apr 2016.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Martin, B. (1992). Scientific fraud and the power structure of science. Prometheus, 10, 83–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Masterman, M. (1999). The nature of a paradigm. In I. Lakatos & A. Musgrave (Eds.), Criticism and the growth of knowledge (pp. 59–90). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  30. Medawar, P. B. (1963). Is the scientific paper a fraud? The Listener, 70, 377–378.Google Scholar
  31. Mehra, J., & Milton, K. A. (2000). Climbing the mountain: The scientific biography of Julian Schwinger. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  32. Morrison, C. (2008, October 21). Blacklight power bolsters its impossible claims of a new renewable energy source. The New York Times. http://www.nytimes.com/external/venturebeat/2008/10/21/21venturebeat-blacklight-power-bolsters-its-impossible-cla-99377.html. Accessed 28 Mar 2016.
  33. Oreskes, N., & Conway, E. M. (2010). Merchants of doubt: How a handful of scientists obscured the truth on issues from tobacco smoke to global warming. New York: Bloomsbury Press.Google Scholar
  34. Pigliucci, M., & Boudry, M. (Eds.). (2013). Philosophy of pseudoscience: Reconsidering the demarcation problem. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  35. Qiu, J. (2010). Publish or perish in China. Nature, 463, 142–143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Redman, B. K. (2013). Research misconduct policy in biomedicine: Beyond the bad-apple approach. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Reich, E. S. (2009). Plastic fantastic: How the biggest fraud in physics shook the scientific world. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.Google Scholar
  38. Richards, R. J., & Daston, L. (2016). Kuhn’s structure of scientific revolutions at fifty: Reflections on a science classic. Chicago: University Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Ritter, S. K. (2012). Reviving cold fusion. Chemical & Engineering News, 90, 42–44.Google Scholar
  40. Rossi A, Leonardo Corporation v. Darden T, John T. Vaughn, Industrial Heat, LLC, IPH International and Cherokee Investment Partners, LLC. (2016). Case 1:16-cv-21199-CMA Document 1. http://www.e-catworld.com/wp-content/uploads/2016/04/Leonardosuit01-main.pdf. Accessed 15 July 2016.
  41. Rouse, J. (2002). Kuhn’s philosophy of scientific practice. In T. Nickles (Ed.), Thomas Kuhn (pp. 101–121). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Schwinger, J. (1991). Cold fusion: Does it have a future? In M. Suzuki & R. Kubo (Eds.), Evolutionary trends in the physical sciences: Proceedings of Yoshio Nishina centennial symposium, Tokyo, 5–7 December 1990. Berlin, Germany: Springer.Google Scholar
  43. Siegel, E. (2016). Is cold fusion feasible? Or is it a fraud? Forbes. https://www.forbes.com/sites/startswithabang/2016/09/23/is-cold-fusion-feasible-or-is-it-a-fraud/#a982d057a050. Accessed 13 Dec 2016.
  44. Singh, S., & Ernst, E. (2008). Trick or treatment: The undeniable facts about alternative medicine. London: Norton.Google Scholar
  45. Stapel, D. (2013). Ontsporing. Amsterdam: Prometheus.Google Scholar
  46. Stogratz, S. (2004). Sync: The emerging science of spontaneous order. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
  47. Suppe, F. (Ed.). (1977). The structure of scientific theories. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
  48. Tallis, R. (2004). Why the mind is not a computer: A pocket lexicon of neuromythology. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  49. Tallis, R. (2012). Aping mankind: Neuromania, darwinitis and the misrepresentation of humanity. Durham, NC: Acumen.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. The Levelt Report. (2012). Flawed science: The fraudulent research practices of social psychologist Diederik Stapel. Levelt Committee, Noort Committee and Drenth Committee. https://www.commissielevelt.nl/. Accessed 10 May 2016.
  51. Thirlwell, J. (2014). A history of anaesthesia journals. In E. Eger, L. Saidman, & R. Westhorpe (Eds.), The wondrous story of anaesthesia (pp. 443–458). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Trubody, B. (2016). The seduction of science: How paradigms can lead one astray. In C. Martins & M. J. Damásio (Eds.), Seduction in popular culture, philosophy and psychology (pp. 1–32). Hershey, PA: IGI Global.Google Scholar
  53. Wicherts, J. M., & Bakker, M. (2014). Broken windows, mediocre methods, and substandard statistics. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 17, 388–403.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Witkowski, T., & Zatonski, M. (2015). Psychology gone wrong: The dark sides of science and therapy. Boca Raton, FL: Brown Walker Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  • Ben Trubody
    • 1
  1. 1.School of Liberal and Performing ArtsUniversity of GloucestershireCheltenhamUK

Personalised recommendations