Advertisement

Infant and Toddler Crying and Irritability: Cultural Meanings and Responses

  • J. Martin Maldonado-DuranEmail author
  • Felipe Lecannelier
Chapter

Abstract

The first ex utero human action is, in most cases, crying—an ethological behavioral response preserved through species, beyond purely mammalian. A response often invoked as a visible sign of life, crying behavior will accompany the newborn through life and represent different meanings and emotions as the individual ages and develops. Beyond the “proof of life,” crying and irritability pose and lead to intense experiences in caregivers that affect neuroendocrine responses. Such reactions are also modulated by the sociocultural circumstances within which the child and caregiver reside. The culture of the caregiver impacts the perception of the crying. These factors also influence the way the infant is attended to and the subsequent responses from his or her growing mind. The caregiver responses vary enormously, in a spectrum that goes from sensitivity to hostility, from benign to malignant. With persistent or excessive crying there is a risk of harming the child. This chapter explores the ethological, neurobiological, and behavioral aspects of infant crying and irritability and provides examples of its modulation within different cultural and social circumstances, including how these affect the perception and interpretation of this otherwise vital action.

Keywords

Crying Irritability Evolutionary psychology Colic Gripe water; Soothing Swaddling Infant carrying Soothing responses Acoustics of crying Shaken baby syndrome 

References

  1. Barr, R. G., Hopkins, B., & Green, J. A. (Eds.). (2000). Crying as a sign, a symptom, and a signal: Clinical, emotional and developmental aspects of infant and toddler crying (No. 152). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  2. Bensel, J. (2003). Frühe Säuglingsunruhe—Einfluss westlicher Betreuungspraktiken und Effekte auf Aktivitätsmuster und biologischen Rhythmus [The influence of Western caregiving practices and their effects on patterns of activity and biological rhythms]. Berlin: VWB.Google Scholar
  3. Bensel, J. (2006). Freiburger Säuglingsstudie: Welche Faktoren beeinflussen das frühe Säuglingsschreien. [Freiburg study of infants. What factors influence early infant crying?]. Die Hebamme, 19(1), 12–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bensel, J. (2010). Exzessives schrien beim säugling. [Excessive crying in the infant]. Die Hebammer, 80–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bolten, M. (2008). Exzessives Schreien [Excessive crying]. Verhaltenstherapie mit Kindern und Jugendlichen, 2, 50–62.Google Scholar
  6. Boyle, E. M., Freer, Y., Khan-Orakzai, Z., Watkinson, M., Wright, E., Ainsworth, J. R., & McIntosh, N. (2006). Sucrose and non-nutritive sucking for the relief of pain in screening for retinopathy of prematurity: A randomised controlled trial. Archives of Disease in Childhood-Fetal and Neonatal Edition, 91(3), F166–F168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Brazelton, T. B. (1977). Implications of infant development among the Mayan Indians of Mexico. In P. Liederman, S. Tulkin, & A. Rosenfeld (Eds.), Culture and infancy: Variations in the human experience (pp. 151–188). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
  8. Brazelton, T. B. (1990). Crying and colic. Infant Mental Health Journal, 11(4), 349–356.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Brazelton, T. B., Robey, J. S., & Collier, G. A. (1969). Infant development in the Zinacanteco Indians of Southern Mexico. Pediatrics, 44(2), 274–290.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Franco, P., Seret, N., Van Hees, J. N., Scaillet, S., Groswasser, J., & Kahn, A. (2005). Influence of swaddling on sleep and arousal characteristics of healthy infants. Pediatrics, 115(5), 1307–1311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Garrison, M. M., & Christakis, D. A. (2000). A systematic review of treatments for infant colic. Pediatrics, 106.(Suppl 1, 184–190.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Gottlieb, A. (1995). Of cowries and crying: A Beng guide to managing colic. Anthropology and Humanism, 20(1), 20–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Gottlieb, A. (1998). Do infants have religion? The spiritual lives of Beng babies. American Anthropologist, 100(1), 122–135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Gottlieb, A. (2016). Luring your child into this life of troubled times. A Beng path for infant care in post-civil war Cote d’Ivoire. In A. Gottlieb & J. DeLoach (Eds.), A world of babies: Imagined childcare for eight societies (pp. 153–190). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  15. Gwandure, C. (2006). Infantile colic among the traditional Shona people: An ethnopsychological perspective. Journal of Psychology in Africa, 16(1), 119–122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Hassenstein, B. (2001). Verhaltensbiologie des Kindes [Behavioral biology of children]. Heidelberg: Spektrum Akademischer Verlag.Google Scholar
  17. Hewlett, B. S., & Lamb, M. E. (2002). Integrating evolution, culture and developmental psychology: Explaining caregiver-infant proximity and responsiveness in Central Africa and the USA. In H. Keller, Y. H. Pooringa, & A. Schoelmerich (Eds.), Between culture and biology. Perspectives on ontogenetic development (pp. 241–269). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hunziker, U. A., & Barr, R. G. (1986). Increased carrying reduces infant crying: A randomized controlled trial. Pediatrics, 77(5), 641–648.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Kivijärvi, M., Voeten, M. J., Niemelä, P., Räihä, H., Lertola, K., & Piha, J. (2001). Maternal sensitivity behavior and infant behavior in early interaction. Infant Mental Health Journal: Official Publication of the World Association for Infant Mental Health, 22(6), 627–640.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Kottler, J. A. (1997). Die Sprache der tränen. Warum wir weinen. [The language of tears. Why we cry]. München: Diana Verlag.Google Scholar
  21. Kramer, M. S., Barr, R. G., Dagenais, S., Yang, H., Jones, P., Ciofani, L., & Jané, F. (2001). Pacifier use, early weaning, and cry/fuss behavior: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA, 286(3), 322–326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Lee, K. (1994). The crying pattern of Korean infants and related factors. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 36, 601–607.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Leng, G., Meddle, S. L., & Douglas, A. J. (2008). Oxytocin and the maternal brain. Current Opinion in Pharmacology, 8, 731–734.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Lucassen, P. L. B. J., Assendelft, W. J. J., Gubbels, J. W., van Eijk, J., Van Geldrop, W. J., & Neven, A. K. (1998). Effectiveness of treatments for infantile colic: Systematic review. BMJ, 316(7144), 1563–1568.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Maldonado-Duran, J. M., Munguía-Wellman, M., Lubin, S., & Lartigue, T. (2002). Latino families in the perinatal period: Cultural issues in dealing with the health-care system. Great Plains Research, 75–100.Google Scholar
  26. Meyer, L. E., & Erler, T. (2011). Swaddling: A traditional care method rediscovered. World Journal of Pediatrics, 7(2), 155–160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Nelson, J. K. (2005). Seeing through tears. Crying and attachment. New York: Taylor & Francis Group.Google Scholar
  28. Nelson, E. A. S., Schiefenhoevel, W., & Haimerl, F. (2000). Child care practices in nonindustrialized societies. Pediatrics, 105(6), e75–e75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Out, D., Pieper, S., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Zeskind, P. S., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2010). Intended sensitive and harsh caregiving responses to infant crying: The role of cry pitch and perceived urgency in an adult twin sample. Child Abuse & Neglect, 34(11), 863–873.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Papousek, M. (2009). Persistierendes Schreien. Schreiprobleme in Entwicklungskontext von Eltern-Kind Kommunikation und Beziehung [Persistent crying. Crying problems in the context of development in the communication and relationship between parent and infant]. Monatschrift für Kinderheilkunde., 157, 558–566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Pinelli, J., & Symington, A. J. (2010). Non-nutritive sucking for promoting physiologic stability and nutrition in preterm infants. Midirs Midwifery Digest, 20(4), 504.Google Scholar
  32. Reijneveld, S. A., van der Wal, M. F., Brugman, E., Sing, R. A., & Verloove-Vanhorick, S. P. (2004). Infant crying and abuse. Lancet, 364(9442), 1340–1342.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Sargent, C. F. (1988). Born to die: Witchcraft and infanticide in Bariba culture. Ethnology, 27(1), 79–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Savino, F., Brondello, C., Cresi, F., Oggero, R., & Silvestro, L. (2002). Cimetropium bromide in the treatment of crisis in infantile colic. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition, 34(4), 417–419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Soltis, J. (2004). The signal functions of early infant crying. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(4), 443–458.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. St James-Roberts, I., Bowyer, J., Varghese, S., & Sawdon, J. (1994). Infant crying patterns in Manali and London. Child: Care, Health and Development, 20(5), 323–337.Google Scholar
  37. St James-Roberts, I., Alvarez, M., Csipke, E., Abramsky, T., Goodwin, J., & Sorgenfrei, E. (2006). Infant crying and sleeping in London, Copenhagen and when parents adopt a “proximal” form of care. Pediatrics, 117(6), e1146–e1155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Swain, J. E. (2011). The human parental brain: In vivo neuroimaging. Progress in Neuro-psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 35, 1242–1254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Swain, J. E., Kim, P., & Ho, S. S. (2011). Neuroendocrinology of parental response to baby‐cry. Journal of neuroendocrinology, 23(11), 1036–1041.Google Scholar
  40. Swain, J. E., Mayes, L. C., & Leckman, J. F. (2004). The development of parent-infant attachment through dynamic and interactive signaling loops of care and cry. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(4), 472–473.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Swain, J. E., Lorberbaum, J. P., Kose, S., & Strathearn, L. (2007). Brain basis of early parent–infant interactions: Psychology, physiology, and in vivo functional neuroimaging studies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(3–4), 262–287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Tarasco, S. F., Lingua, C., Moja, L., & Ricceri, F. (2012). Pain-relieving agents for colic. The Cochran Collaboration 2012. Wiley.Google Scholar
  43. Trevathan, W. R., & McKenna, J. J. (1994). Evolutionary environments of human birth and infancy: Insights to apply to contemporary life. Children’s Environments, 88–104.Google Scholar
  44. Van den Bergh, B. (2005). Über die Folgen negativer müttlicher Emotionalität während der Schwangerschaft [On the consequences of negative emotionality in the mother during pregnancy]. In I. Krens & H. Krens (Eds.), Grundlagen einer vorgerburtliche Psychologie [The basics of a psychology before birth] (pp. 94–105). Göttingen: Vandenhoeck & Ruprecht.Google Scholar
  45. Vik, T., Grote, V., Escribano, J., et al. (2009). Infantile colic, prolonged crying and maternal postnatal depression. Acta Paediatrica, 98, 1344–1348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Wehrstedt, C. (2011). Kann exzessives Schrien durch Tragen beeinflusst werden? [Can excessive crying be influence by carrying?]. Die Hebamme, 2011, 252–258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Zahr, L. K., & Hattar-Pollara, M. (1998). Nursing care of Arab children: Consideration of cultural factors. Journal of Pediatric Nursing, 13(6), 349–355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Zeifman, D. M. (2001). An ethological analysis of human infant crying: Answering Tinbergen’s four questions. Developmental Psychobiology: The Journal of the International Society for Developmental Psychobiology, 39(4), 265–285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  • J. Martin Maldonado-Duran
    • 1
    Email author
  • Felipe Lecannelier
    • 2
  1. 1.Menninger Department of PsychiatryBaylor College of MedicineHoustonUSA
  2. 2.Facultad de Ciencias MedicasUniversidad de Santiago de ChileSantiagoChile

Personalised recommendations