Advertisement

Outlook

  • Wolfgang Tschacher
  • Hermann Haken
Chapter

Abstract

In this outlook chapter we discuss what was accomplished by the Fokker–Planck modeling of psychotherapy processes, and suggest directions for future research. We emphasize that psychotherapy research should focus on the dynamics, preferably using time series with high resolution which delivers insight into the dynamics even within the range of a few seconds, the “nowness” of embodied consciousness. A further prominent issue is therapeutic coupling and entrainment using measures of synchrony, where there is need to further standardize the various computational approaches. We claim that therapeutic intervention may be achieved by the “Archimedean role” of the therapist, who obtains leverage through his or her personality. These are implications and conclusions based on the minimal models developed in previous chapters. When focusing on the affordances of the therapeutic setting, we arrive at a new interpretation of the contextual model: contextual-deterministic interventions utilize especially the motivational common factors, such as resource activation or cognitive dissonance, in order to modify the attractor landscape of a client.

References

  1. Altmann, U. (2013). Synchronisation nonverbalen Verhaltens. Weiterentwicklung und Anwendung zeitreihenanalytischer Identifikationsverfahren. Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bruineberg, J., & Rietveld, E. (2014). Self-organization, free energy minimization, and optimal grip on a field of affordances. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8(599).Google Scholar
  3. Buszáki, G. (2006). Rhythms of the brain. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Chalmers, D. (1996). The conscious mind. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  5. Dehaene, S. (2014). Consciousness and the brain. Deciphering how the brain codes our thoughts. New York, NY: Viking Press.Google Scholar
  6. Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
  7. Friston, K. J. (2010). The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 127–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Friston, K. J. (2011). Embodied inference: Or “I think therefore I am, if I am what I think”. In W. Tschacher & C. Bergomi (Eds.), The implications of embodiment – Cognition and communication (pp. 89–125). Exeter, UK: Imprint Academic.Google Scholar
  9. Fuchs, T. (2016). Self across time: The diachronic unity of bodily existence. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 15, 1–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
  11. Granada, A. E., & Herzel, H. (2009). How to achieve fast entrainment? The timescale to synchronization. PLoS ONE, 4(e7057).Google Scholar
  12. Grawe, K. (2004). Psychological therapy. Seattle, WA: Hogrefe and Huber.Google Scholar
  13. Grinder, J., & Bandler, R. (1981). Trance-formations. Moab, UT: Real People Press.Google Scholar
  14. Haken, H. (1977). Synergetics — An introduction. Nonequilibrium phase-transitions and self-organization in physics, chemistry and biology. Berlin, Germany: Springer.Google Scholar
  15. Haken, H., & Portugali, J. (2016). Information and self-organization: A unifying approach and applications. Entropy, 18, 197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Haken, H., & Tschacher, W. (2010). A theoretical model of intentionality with an application to neural dynamics. Mind and Matter, 8, 7–18.Google Scholar
  17. Karvonen, A., Kykyri, V.-L., Kaartinen, J., Penttonen, M., & Seikkula, J. (2016). Sympathetic nervous system synchrony in couple therapy. Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 42, 383–395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Kotchoubey, B., Tretter, F., Braun, H. A., Buchheim, T., Draguhn, A., Fuchs, T., … Tschacher, W. (2016). Methodological problems on the way to integrative human neuroscience. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 10(41).Google Scholar
  19. Kupper, Z., Ramseyer, F., Hoffmann, H., & Tschacher, W. (2015). Nonverbal synchrony in social interactions of patients with schizophrenia indicates socio-communicative deficits. PLoS ONE, 10(e0145882).Google Scholar
  20. Kyselo, M. (2014). The body social: An enactive approach to the self. Frontiers in Cognitive Science, 5(986).  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00986
  21. Lewin, K. (1936). Principles of topological psychology. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Linden, M. (2013). How to define, find and classify side effects in psychotherapy: From unwanted events to adverse treatment reactions. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 20, 286–296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Lutz, W., & Barkham, M. (2015). Therapist effects. In R. L. Cautin & S. O. Lilienfeld (Eds.), The encyclopedia of clinical psychology (pp. 1–6). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
  24. Martin, L. A., Koch, S. C., Hirjak, D., & Fuchs, T. (2016). Overcoming disembodiment: The effect of movement therapy on negative symptoms in schizophrenia - A multicenter randomized controlled trial. Frontiers in Psychology, 7(483).  https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00483
  25. Moulder, R. G., Boker, S. M., Ramseyer, F., & Tschacher, W. (2018). Determining synchrony between behavioral time series: An application of surrogate data generation for establishing falsifiable null-hypotheses. Psychological Methods, 23, 757–773.  https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000172CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Nicolis, G., & Prigogine, I. (1977). Self-organization in nonequilibrium systems: From dissipative structures to order through fluctuations. New York, NY: Wiley-Interscience.Google Scholar
  27. Paulick, J., Deisenhofer, A.-K., Ramseyer, F., Tschacher, W., Boyle, K., Rubel, J., & Lutz, W. (2018). Nonverbal synchrony: A new approach to better understand psychotherapeutic processes and drop-out. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 28, 367–384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Pöppel, E. (1997). A hierarchical model of temporal perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1, 56–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Ramseyer, F., & Tschacher, W. (2011). Nonverbal synchrony in psychotherapy: Coordinated body movement reflects relationship quality and outcome. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 79, 284–295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Ramseyer, F., & Tschacher, W. (2016). Movement coordination in psychotherapy: Synchrony of hand movements is associated with session outcome. A single-case study. Nonlinear Dynamics, Psychology, and Life Sciences, 20, 145–166.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  31. Röhricht, F. (2009). Body oriented psychotherapy. The state of the art in empirical research and evidence-based practice: A clinical perspective. Body Movement and Dance in Psychotherapy, 4, 135–156.  https://doi.org/10.1080/17432970902857263CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Rossi, E. L. (Ed.). (1980). The nature of hypnosis and suggestion (collected papers of Milton H. Erickson, Vol. 1). New York, NY: Irvington Publishers.Google Scholar
  33. Stern, D. N. (2004). The present moment in psychotherapy and everyday life. New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar
  34. Stukenbrock, A. (2018). Forward-looking. Where do we go with multimodal projections? In A. Deppermann & J. Streeck (Eds.), Time in Embodied Interaction. Synchronicity and sequentiality of multimodal resources (pp. 31–68). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Thomson, J. M. T., & Stewart, H. B. (2002). Nonlinear dynamics and chaos (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
  36. Tschacher, W., & Haken, H. (2007a). Intentionality in non-equilibrium systems? The functional aspects of self-organised pattern formation. New Ideas in Psychology, 25, 1–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Tschacher, W., & Haken, H. (2007b). Intentionality in non-equilibrium systems? The functional aspects of self-organized pattern formation. New Ideas in Psychology, 25, 1–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Tschacher, W., & Pfammatter, M. (2016). Embodiment in psychotherapy – A necessary complement to the canon of common factors? European Psychotherapy, 13, 9–25.Google Scholar
  39. Tschacher, W., & Ramseyer, F. (2009). Modeling psychotherapy process by time-series panel analysis (TSPA). Psychotherapy Research, 19, 469–481.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Tschacher, W., Ramseyer, F., & Koole, S. L. (2018). Sharing the now in the social present: Duration of nonverbal synchrony is linked with personality. Journal of Personality, 86, 129–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Tschacher, W., & Rössler, O. (1996). The self: A processual Gestalt. Chaos, Solitons & Fractals, 7, 1011–1022.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Varela, F. J. (1999). Present-time consciousness. In F. J. Varela & J. Shear (Eds.), The view from within: First-person methodologies (pp. 111–140). Exeter, UK: Imprint Academic.Google Scholar
  43. Varga, S., & Heck, D. H. (2017). Rhythms of the body, rhythms of the brain: Respiration, neural oscillations, and embodied cognition. Consciousness and Cognition, 56, 77–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Wampold, B. E. (2015). How important are the common factors in psychotherapy? An update. World Psychiatry, 14, 270–277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Zhang, Y., Meng, T., Hou, Y., Pan, Y., & Hu, Y. (2018). Interpersonal brain synchronization associated with working alliance during psychological counseling. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 282, 103–109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  • Wolfgang Tschacher
    • 1
    • 2
  • Hermann Haken
    • 3
  1. 1.Universitätsklinik für Psychiatrie und PsychotherapieUniversitäre Psychiatrische, Dienste BernBernSwitzerland
  2. 2.Freiburg Institute for Advanced Studies (FRIAS)Universität FreiburgFreiburgGermany
  3. 3.Institut für Theoretische Physik und SynergetikUniversität StuttgartStuttgartGermany

Personalised recommendations