Advertisement

In Quest of Meaningful Change

  • James T. LamiellEmail author
Chapter
Part of the Palgrave Studies in the Theory and History of Psychology book series (PSTHP)

Abstract

This final chapter begins with a brief review of the major historical developments, elaborated in previous chapters, that landed mainstream psychology in its current epistemic predicament. I then discuss a variety of matters about which maximum clarity will be essential if the obdurate resistance that has thus far prevailed within psychology’s mainstream to the needed change in its investigative methods is ever finally to be overcome. First, I note that although psycho-demographic inquiry cannot qualify as psychology, I explain both that and why this is not an argument that such inquiry is of no merit. Second, and against the notion that no viable alternatives to the currently dominant research paradigm exist, attention is directed to a promising framework called ‘observation oriented modeling.’ Third, I emphasize further that the present argument is not against the use of quantitative methods in psychology per se, but is rather against the use of aggregate statistical methods as a way of advancing knowledge of the psychological functioning of individuals. Fourth, I discuss the need to overcome enduring misconceptions in contemporary psychologists’ understanding of the concepts of ‘nomothetic’ and ‘idiographic.’ The chapter concludes with a plea for reviving a conception of psychology as both a natural science and a human science, noting that such a revival will bring with it a recognition of the need for and scientific legitimacy of both quantitative and qualitative research methods.

References

  1. Bakan, D. (1955). The general and the aggregate: A methodological distinction. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 5, 211–212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Bakan, D. (1966). The test of significance in psychological research. Psychological Bulletin, 66, 423–437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Barlow, D. H., & Nock, M. K. (2009). Why can’t we be more idiographic in our research? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 19–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Cronbach, L. J. (1957). The two disciplines of scientific psychology. American Psychologist, 12, 671–684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Dilthey, W. (1894). Ideen über eine beschreibende und zergliedernde Psychologie [Ideas concerning a descriptive and an analytical psychology]. Sitzungsberichte der Akademie der Wissenschaften zu Berlin. Zweiter Halbband, 1309–1407.Google Scholar
  6. Edwards, W., Lindman, H., & Savage, L. (1963). Bayesian statistical inference for psychological research. Psychological Review, 70, 193–242.  https://doi.org/10.1037/h0044139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Gantt, E. E., & Williams, R. N. (Eds.). 2018. On hijacking science: Exploring the nature and consequences of overreach in psychology. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
  8. Grice, J. W. (2011). Observation oriented modeling: Analysis of cause in the behavioral sciences. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  9. Grice, J. W. (2014). Observation oriented modeling. Comprehensive Psychology, 3, ISSN 2165-2228.Google Scholar
  10. Grice, J. W. (2015). From means and variances to persons and patterns. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Grice, J., Barrett, P., Cota, L., Felix, C., Taylor, Z., Garner, S., et al. (2017). Four bad habits of modern psychologists. Behavioral sciences, 7, 1–21.  https://doi.org/10.3390/bs7030053.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Grice, J. W., Barrett, P. T., Schlimgen, L. A., & Abramson, C. I. (2012). Toward a brighter future for psychology as an observation oriented science. Behavioral sciences, 2, 1–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Harré, R. (2006). Key thinkers in psychology. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
  14. Johnson, A. D., Markowitz, A. J., Hill, C. J., & Phillips, D. A. (2016). Variatioin in impacts of Tulsa pre-K on cognitive development in kindergarten: The role of instructional support. Developmental Psychology, 52, 2145–2158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Journal for Person-Oriented Research, 2015, Volume 1, statement of Aims and Scope. ISSN 2002-0244.Google Scholar
  16. Kerlinger, F. N. (1979). Behavioral research: A conceptual approach. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.Google Scholar
  17. Lamiell, J. T. (1981). Toward an idiothetic psychology of personality. American Psychologist, 36, 276–289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Lamiell, J. T. (1998). “Nomothetic” and “idiographic”: Contrasting Windelband’s understanding with contemporary usage. Theory and Psychology, 10, 715–730.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Lamiell, J. T. (2018a, forthcoming). From psychology to psychodemography: How the adoption of population-level statistical methods transformed psychological science. American Journal of Psychology, 131, 471–475.Google Scholar
  20. Lamiell, J. T. (2018b). Rejoinder to Proctor and Xiong. American Journal of Psychology, 131, 489–492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Lamiell, J. T., & Durbeck, P. (1987). Whence cognitive prototypes in impression formation? Some empirical evidence for dialectical reasoning as a generative process. Journal of Mind and Behavior, 8, 223–244.Google Scholar
  22. Lamiell, J. T., Foss, M. A., Larsen, R. J., & Hempel, A. (1983). Studies in intuitive personology from an idiothetic point of view: Implications for personality theory. Journal of Personality, 51, 438–467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Lilienfeld, S. O., Ritschel, L. A., Lynn, S. J., Cautin, R. L., & Latzman, R. D. (2013). Why many clinical psychologists are resistant to evidence-based practice: Root causes and constructive remedies. Clinical Psychology Review, 33, 883–900.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Machado, A., & Silva, F. J. (2007). Toward a richer view of the scientific method: The role of conceptual analysis. American Psychologist, 62, 671–681.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.62.7.671.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  25. Porter, T. M. (1986). The rise of statistical thinking: 1820–1900. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  26. Proctor, R. W., & Xiong, A. (2018). Adoption of population-level statistical methods did transform psychological science but for the better: Commentary on Lamiell (2018). American Journal of Psychology, 131, 483–487.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Schiff, B. (2017). A new narrative for psychology. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Schiff, B. (Ed.). (2018). Situating qualitative methods in psychological science. Abingdon, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
  29. Williams, R. N., & Robinson, D. N. (Eds.). (2016). Scientism: The new orthodoxy. New York: Bloomsbury Publishing.Google Scholar
  30. Windelband, W. (1894/1998). History and natural science (J. T. Lamiell, Trans.). Theory and Psychology, 8, 6–22.Google Scholar
  31. Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., & Mather, N. (2001). Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Achievement. Itasca, IL: Riverside.Google Scholar
  32. Wundt, W. (1913/2013). Psychology’s struggle for existence (J. T. Lamiell, Trans.). History of Psychology, 16, 195–209.  https://doi.org/10.1037/0032319.

Copyright information

© The Author(s) 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Georgetown UniversityWashingtonUSA

Personalised recommendations