Advances in Psychology and Law pp 101-135 | Cite as
Post-identification Feedback to Eyewitnesses: Implications for System Variable Reform
Abstract
Eyewitness memory can be distorted by simple comments received after an identification decision is made. When these comments suggest that the identification decision was correct, they inflate witnesses’ recollections of how confident they were, how good their view was, and other testimony-relevant judgments. This post-identification feedback effect is a robust phenomenon with significant implications for judging the reliability of eyewitness evidence. For example, research showing particularly powerful effects of feedback on witnesses who have made mistaken decisions presents a significant risk to wrongly identified people. In the current chapter, we begin with an overview of 20 years of research on the feedback effect. Next, we analyze how feedback research has factored into two recent state supreme court decisions and a U.S. Supreme Court decision. After reviewing the court decisions, we discuss the potential for feedback research to both strengthen and refine system variable reforms as outlined in the 2017 Department of Justice memorandum on eyewitness identification procedures. Finally, we present future research suggestions including the imperative to study how feedback might emerge in new ways (e.g., through witnesses’ own “investigations” using social media).
Keywords
Post-identification feedback Eyewitness decisions Confidence malleability Retrospective confidenceNotes
Acknowledgements
We thank the Bates College Summer Research Apprentice Program and Paola Herrera for assistance in preparing this chapter. Portions of work on this chapter were supported by the National Science Foundation under Grant No. SES-1627433 to Amy Douglass and Neil Brewer. Any opinions, findings, and conclusions or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the views of the National Science Foundation.
References
- Alvarez, M. J., Miller, M. K., & Bornstein, B. H. (2016). ‘It will be your duty…:’ The psychology of criminal jury instructions. In M. K. Miller & B. H. Bornstein (Eds.), Advances in psychology and law (pp. 119–158). New York, NY: Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-29406-3_4.Google Scholar
- Beaudry, J. L., Lindsay, R. C. L., Leach, A., Mansour, J. K., Bertrand, M. I., & Kalmet, N. (2015). The effect of evidence type, identification accuracy line-up presentation, and line-up administration on observers’ perceptions of eyewitnesses. Legal & Criminological Psychology, 20, 343–364. https://doi.org/10.1111/lcrp.12030.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bem, D. J. (1967). Self-perception: An alternative interpretation of the cognitive dissonance phenomena. Psychological Review, 74, 183–200. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0024835.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bhaskara, A., Semmler, C., Brewer, N., & Douglass, A. B. (2016). Eyewitnesses’ objective judgments about viewing time and distance can be distorted by post-identification information. Manuscript under review.Google Scholar
- Bradfield, A., & Wells, G. L. (2005). Not the same old hindsight bias: Outcome information distorts a broad range of retrospective judgments. Memory & Cognition, 33, 120–130. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bradfield, A. L., Wells, G. L., & Olson, E. A. (2002). The damaging effect of confirming feedback on the relation between eyewitness certainty and identification accuracy. Journal of Applied Psychology, 87, 112–120. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.87.1.112.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Brooks, W. G. (2017). U.S. Identification Procedures. Eyewitness Identification Workshop, Boston, MA.Google Scholar
- Carlson, C. A., Gronlund, S. D., & Clark, S. E. (2008). Lineup composition, suspect position, and the sequential lineup advantage. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 14, 118–128. https://doi.org/10.1037/1076-898X.14.2.118.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Cash, D. K., & Lane, S. M. (2017). Context influences interpretation of eyewitness confidence statements. Law and Human Behavior, 41, 180–190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Charman, S. D., Carlucci, M., Vallano, J., & Gregory, A. H. (2010). The selective cue integration framework: A theory of postidentification witness confidence assessment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 16, 204–218. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019495.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Charman, S. D., & Quiroz, V. (2016). Blind sequential lineup administration reduces both false identifications and confidence in those false identifications. Law and Human Behavior, 40, 477–487. https://doi.org/10.1037/lhb0000197.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Charman, S. D., & Wells, G. L. (2008). Can eyewitnesses correct for external influences on their lineup identifications? The actual/counterfactual assessment paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 14, 5–20. http://dx.doi.org.lprx.bates.edu/10.1037/1076-898X.14.1.5.
- Clark, S. E. (2005). A re-examination of the effects of biased lineup instructions in eyewitness identification. Law and Human Behavior, 29, 575–604. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-005-7121-1.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Clark, S. E. (2012). Eyewitness identification reform: Data, theory, and due process. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 279–283. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612444136.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Clark, S. E., & Davey, S. L. (2005). The target-to-foils shift in simultaneous and sequential lineups. Law and Human Behavior, 29, 151–172. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-005-2418-7.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Dodson, C. S., & Dobolyi, D. G. (2015). Misinterpreting eyewitness expressions of confidence: The featural justification effect. Law and Human Behavior, 39, 266–280. https://doi.org/10.1037/lhb0000120.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Douglass, A. B., Brewer, N., & Semmler, C. (2010a). Moderators of post-identification feedback effects on eyewitnesses’ memory reports. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 15, 279–292. https://doi.org/10.1348/135532509X446337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Douglass, A. B., & Jones, E. E. (2013). Confidence inflation in eyewitnesses: Seeing is not believing. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 18, 152–167. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8333.2011.02031.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Douglass, A. B., & McQuiston-Surrett, D. (2006). Post-identification feedback: Exploring the effects of sequential photospreads and eyewitnesses’ awareness of the identification task. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 991–1007. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Douglass, A. B., Neuschatz, J. S., Imrich, J. F., & Wilkinson, M. (2010b). Does post-identification feedback affect evaluations of eyewitness testimony and identification procedures? Law and Human Behavior, 34, 282–294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Douglass, A. B., & Steblay, N. (2006). Memory distortion in eyewitnesses: A meta-analysis of the post-identification feedback effect. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 859–869. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Dysart, J. E., Lawson, V. Z., & Rainey, A. (2012). Blind lineup administration as a prophylactic against the post identification feedback effect. Law and Human Behavior, 36, 312–319. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0093921.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Edwards, K., & Smith, E. E. (1996). A disconfirmation bias in the evaluation of arguments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 5–24. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.71.1.5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Fitzgerald, R. J., Price, H. L., Oriet, C., & Charman, S. D. (2013). The effect of suspect-filler similarity on eyewitness identification decisions: A meta-analysis. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 19, 151–164. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030618.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Garrett, B. L. (2011). Convicting the innocent: Where criminal prosecutions go wrong. Cambridge, MA, USA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Garrioch, L., & Brimacombe, C. E. (2001). Lineup administrators’ expectations: Their impact on eyewitness confidence. Law and Human Behavior, 25, 299–315. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1010750028643.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Greathouse, S. M., & Kovera, M. B. (2009). Instruction bias and lineup presentation moderate the effects of administrator knowledge on eyewitness identification. Law and Human Behavior, 33, 70–82. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-008-9136-x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Hafstad, G. S., Memon, A., & Logie, R. (2004). Post-identification feedback, confidence, and recollections of witnessing conditions in child witnesses. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18, 901–912. http://dx.doi.org.lprx.bates.edu/10.1002/acp.1037.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Jones, A. M., Bergold, A. N., Dillon, M. K., & Penrod, S. D. (2017). Comparing the effectiveness of Henderson instructions and expert testimony: Which safeguard improves jurors’ evaluations of eyewitness evidence? Journal of Experimental Criminology, 13, 29–52. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11292-016-9279-6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Jones, E. E., Williams, K. D., & Brewer, N. (2008). ‘I had a confidence epiphany!’: Obstacles to combating post-identification confidence inflation. Law and Human Behavior, 32, 164–176. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-007-9101-0.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Kassin, S. M. (1985). Eyewitness identification: Retrospective self-awareness and the accuracy-confidence correlation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 878–893. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.49.4.878.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kassin, S. M. (1998). Eyewitness identification procedures: The fifth rule. Law and Human Behavior, 22, 649–653. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025702722645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kassin, S. M., Kukucka, J., Lawson, V. Z., & DeCarlo, J. (2014). Does video recording alter the behavior of police during interrogation? A mock crime-and-investigation study. Law and Human Behavior, 38, 73–83. https://doi.org/10.1037/lhb0000047.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Kassin, S. M., Rigby, S., & Castillo, S. R. (1991). The accuracy-confidence correlation in eyewitness testimony: Limits and extensions of the retrospective self-awareness effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 598–707. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.61.5.698.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Lampinen, J. M., Scott, J., Pratt, D., Leding, J. K., & Arnal, J. D. (2007). ‘Good, you identified the suspect…but please ignore this feedback’: Can warnings eliminate the effects of post-identification feedback? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 21, 1037–1056. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Lindsay, R. C., & Wells, G. L. (1985). Improving eyewitness identifications from lineups: Simultaneous versus sequential lineup presentation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 70, 556–564. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.70.3.556.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Lord, C. G., Ross, L., & Lepper, M. R. (1979). Biased assimilation and attitude polarization: The effects of prior theories on subsequently considered evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 2098–2109. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.37.11.2098.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Manson v. Brathwaite, 432 U.S. 98, 97 S.Ct. 2243, 53 L. Ed.2d 140 (1977).Google Scholar
- National Research Council. (2014). Identifying the culprit: Assessing eyewitness identification. Washington, D.C.: The National Academies Press. https://doi.org/10.17226/18891.
- Neil v. Biggers, 409 U.S. 188, 199, 93 S.Ct. 375, 382, 34 L. Ed.2d 401, 411 (1972).Google Scholar
- Neuschatz, J. S., Lawson, D. S., Fairless, A. H., Powers, R. A., Neuschatz, J. S., Goodsell, C. A., et al. (2007). The mitigating effects of suspicion on post-identification feedback and on retrospective eyewitness memory. Law and Human Behavior, 31, 231–247. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-006-9047-7.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Neuschatz, J. S., Preston, E. L., Burkett, A. D., Toglia, M. P., Lampinen, J. M., Neuschatz, J. S., et al. (2005). The effects of post-identification feedback and age on retrospective eyewitness memory. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19, 435–453. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1084.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- New Jersey v. Henderson, 27 A. 3d 872 (2011).Google Scholar
- Oregon v. Classen 590 P.2d 1198. (1979). 285 Or. 221.Google Scholar
- Oregon v. Lawson, SC S059306 (2012).Google Scholar
- Palmer, M. A., Brewer, N., & Weber, N. (2010). Postidentification feedback affects subsequent eyewitness identification performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 16, 387–398. http://dx.doi.org.lprx.bates.edu/10.1037/a0021034.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Papailiou, A. P., Yokum, D. V., & Robertson, C. T. (2015). The novel New Jersey eyewitness instruction induces skepticism but not sensitivity. PLoS One, 10. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0142695.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Pastore, A. L., & Maguire, K. (2003). Sourcebook of criminal justice statistics. U.S. Department of Justice (Eds.), Bureau of Justice Statistics. Washington, D.C.: USGPO.Google Scholar
- Paterson, H. M., & Kemp, R. I. (2006). Comparing methods of encountering post-event information: The power of co-witness suggestion. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 20, 1083–1099. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Perry v. New Hampshire. 565 U.S. 132 S. Ct. 716, 181 L. Ed. 2d 694 (2012).Google Scholar
- Phillips, M. R., McAuliff, B. D., Kovera, M. B., & Cutler, B. L. (1999). Double-blind photoarray administration as a safeguard against investigator bias. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84, 940–951. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.84.6.940.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Police Executive Research Forum. (2013). A National survey of eyewitness identification procedures in law enforcement agencies. Report submitted to the National Institute of Justice, March 8, 2013.Google Scholar
- Quinlivan, D. S., Neuschatz, J. S., Cutler, B. L., Wells, G. L., McClung, J., & Harker, D. L. (2012). Do pre-admonition suggestions moderate the effect of unbiased lineup instructions? Legal and Criminological Psychology, 17, 165–176. https://doi.org/10.1348/135532510X533554.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Quinlivan, D. S., Neuschatz, J. S., Jimenez, A., Cling, A. D., Douglass, A. B., & Goodsell, C. A. (2009). Do prophylactics prevent inflation? Post-identification feedback and the effectiveness of procedures to protect against confidence-inflation in ear-witnesses. Law and Human Behavior, 33, 111–121. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-008-9132-1.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Quinlivan, D. S., Wells, G. L., & Neuschatz, J. S. (2010). Is manipulative intent necessary to mitigate the eyewitness post-identification feedback effect? Law and Human Behavior, 34, 186–197. http://dx.doi.org.lprx.bates.edu/10.1007/s10979-009-9179-7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Reardon, M. C., & Fisher, R. P. (2011). Effect of viewing the interview and identification process on juror perceptions on eyewitness accuracy. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25, 68–77. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1643.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Sauer, J., & Brewer, N. (2015). Confidence and accuracy of eyewitness identification. In T. Valentine & J. Davis (Eds.), Forensic facial identification: Theory and practice of identification from eyewitnesses, composites, and CCTV (pp. 185–208). London: Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
- Semmler, C., Brewer, N., & Wells, G. L. (2004). Effects of postidentification feedback on eyewitness identification and nonidentification confidence. Journal of Applied Psychology, 89, 334–346. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.89.2.334.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Shaw, J. S., III, Appio, L. M., Zerr, T. K., & Pontoski, L. E. (2007). Public eyewitness confidence can be influenced by the presence to other witnesses. Law and Human Behavior, 31, 629–652. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-006-9080-6.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Skagerberg, E. M. (2007). Co-witness feedback in line-ups. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 21, 489–497. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Skagerberg, E. M., & Wright, D. B. (2008). The prevalence of co-witnesses and co-witness discussions in real eyewitnesses. Psychology, Crime, & Law, 14, 513–521. https://doi.org/10.1080/10683160801948980.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Skagerberg, E. M., & Wright, D. B. (2009). Susceptibility to postidentification feedback is affected by source credibility. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 506–523. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Smalarz, L., Greathouse, S. M., Wells, G. L., & Newirth, K. A. (2016). Psychological science on eyewitness identification and the U.S. Supreme Court: Reconsiderations in light of DNA exonerations and the science of eyewitness identification. In C. Willis-Esqueda, R. Wiener, & B. Bornstein (Eds.), The witness stand and Lawrence S. Wrightsman, Jr. (pp. 17–39). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Smalarz, L., Scherr, K. C., & Kassin, S. M. (2016b). Miranda at 50: A psychological analysis. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25, 455–460. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721416665097.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Smalarz, L., & Wells, G. L. (2014a). Confirming feedback following a mistaken identification impairs memory for the culprit. Law and Human Behavior, 38, 283–292. https://doi.org/10.1037/lhb0000078.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Smalarz, L., & Wells, G. L. (2014b). Post-identification feedback to eyewitnesses impairs evaluators’ abilities to discriminate between accurate and mistaken testimony. Law and Human Behavior, 38, 194–202. https://doi.org/10.1037/lhb0000067.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Smalarz, L., & Wells, G. L. (2015). Contamination of eyewitness self-reports and the mistaken-identification problem. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24, 120–124. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721414554394.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Sommers, S. R., & Douglass, A. B. (2007). Context matters: Alibi strength varies according to evaluator perspective. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 12, 41–54. https://doi.org/10.1348/135532506X114301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Steblay, N. M. (1997). Social influence in eyewitness recall: A meta-analytic review of lineup instruction effects. Law and Human Behavior, 21, 283–297. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1024890732059.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Steblay, N. K., Dysart, J. E., & Wells, G. L. (2011). Seventy-two tests of the sequential lineup superiority effect: A meta-analysis and policy discussion. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 17, 99–139. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021650.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Steblay, N. K., Wells, G. L., & Douglass, A. B. (2014). The eyewitness post identification feedback effect 15 years later: Theoretical and policy implications. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 20, 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1037/law000001.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Supreme Judicial Court Study Group on Eyewitness Evidence: Report and Recommendations to the Justices. (2013). Downloaded from http://www.mass.gov/courts/docs/sjc/docs/eyewitness-evidence-report-2013.pdf (9/8/17).
- Technical Working Group for Eyewitness Evidence. (1999). Eyewitness evidence: A guide for law enforcement. Washington, D.C: United States Department of Justice, Office of Justice Programs.Google Scholar
- Thompson-Cannino, J., Cotton, R., & Torneo, E. (2009). Picking cotton: Our memoir of injustice and redemption. New York: St. Martin’s Press.Google Scholar
- United States v. Wade, 388 U.S. 218, 87 S.Ct. 1926, 18 L. Ed.2d 1149 (1967).Google Scholar
- United States Department of Justice (2017, January 6). Eyewitness identification: Procedures for conducting photo arrays. Memorandum for heads of department law enforcement components all department prosecutors.Google Scholar
- Wells, G. L. (1978). Applied eyewitness-testimony research: System variables and estimator variables. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 1546–1557. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.36.12.1546.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Wells, G. L. (1993). What do we know about eyewitness identification? American Psychologist, 48, 553–571. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.48.5.553CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Wells, G. L., & Bradfield, A. L. (1998). “Good, you identified the suspect”: Feedback to eyewitnesses distorts their reports of the witnessing experience. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83, 360–376. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.83.3.360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Wells, G. L., & Bradfield, A. L. (1999). Measuring the goodness of lineups: Parameter estimation, question effects, and limits to the mock witness paradigm. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 13, S27–S39. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(199911)13:1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Wells, G. L., Greathouse, S. M., & Smalarz, L. (2012). Why do motions to suppress suggestive eyewitness identifications fail? In B. L. Cutler (Ed.), Conviction of the innocent: Lessons from psychological research (pp. 167–184). Washington, D.C., US: American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/13085-008.
- Wells, G. L., Lindsay, R. C., & Ferguson, T. J. (1979). Accuracy, confidence, and juror perceptions in eyewitness identification. Journal of Applied Psychology, 64, 440–448. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.64.4.440.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Wells, G. L., & Quinlivan, D. S. (2009). The eyewitness post-identification feedback effect: What is the function of flexible confidence estimates for autobiographical events? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 1153–1163. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1616.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Wells, G. L., & Quigley-McBride, A. (2016). Applying eyewitness identification research to the legal system: A glance at where we have been and where we could go. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 5, 290–294. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2016.07.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Wells, G. L., Steblay, N. K., & Dysart, J. E. (2012b). Eyewitness identification reforms: Are suggestiveness-induced hits and guesses true hits? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 7, 264–271. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691612443368.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Wells, G. L., Steblay, N. K., & Dysart, J. E. (2015a). Double-blind photo lineups using actual eyewitnesses: An experimental test of a sequential versus simultaneous lineup procedure. Law and Human Behavior, 39, 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1037/lhb0000096.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Wells, G. L., Small, M., Penrod, S., Malpass, R. S., Fulero, S. M., & Brimacombe, C. A. E. (1998). Eyewitness identification procedures: Recommendations for lineups and photospreads. Law and Human Behavior, 22, 603–647. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025750605807.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Wells, G. L., Yang, Y., & Smalarz, L. (2015b). Eyewitness identification: Bayesian information gain, base rate effect equivalency curves, and reasonable suspicion. Law and Human Behavior, 39, 99–122. https://doi.org/10.1037/lhb0000125.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
- Wilford, M. M., & Wells, G. L. (2013). Eyewitness system variables. In B. L. Cutler & B. L. Cutler (Eds.), Reform of eyewitness identification procedures (pp. 23–43). Washington, D.C., US: American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/14094-002.
- Wise, R. A., & Safer, M. A. (2010). A comparison of what U.S. judges and students know and believe about eyewitness testimony. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 40, 1400–1422. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2010.00623.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Wright, D. B., & McDaid, A. T. (1996). Comparing system and estimator variables using data from real line-ups. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 10, 75–84. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0720(199602)10:1%3c75:AID-ACP364%3e3.0.CO;2-E.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Wright, D. B., & Skagerberg, E. M. (2007). Postidentification feedback affects real eyewitnesses. Psychological Science, 18, 172–178. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01868.x.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar