Advertisement

Expert Intelligence: Theory of the Missing Facet

  • Jabez Christopher
  • Rajendra Prasath
  • Odelu VangaEmail author
Conference paper
Part of the Lecture Notes in Computer Science book series (LNCS, volume 11308)

Abstract

This article sheds light on various less-explored areas, such as knowledge, intelligence, expertise, knowledge representation, skill acquisition and intelligent systems. It also proposes a missing facet, namely, Expert Intelligence (EI). Artificial Intelligence (AI) based systems such as expert systems and decision support systems were meant to substitute the expertise of human experts. However, in reality, these so-called intelligent systems are used as an aid for decision-making or only as a second choice of opinion in the absence of an expert. During the design of an intelligent system, a knowledge engineer encodes the knowledge of a domain expert into the system. The design and architecture of the system is meant to manipulate on the knowledge of the domain expert but his intelligence is neither acquired nor manifested. Furthermore, poor knowledge acquisition and knowledge representation schemes penalize the performance of these systems. Intelligent systems require more of an experts intelligence rather than his knowledge. Expert Intelligence attempts to bridge this gap. The notion of this article is not to provide an experimental analysis; the principle contribution of this work includes the dogma of Expert Intelligence and future directions for a paradigm-shift from knowledge-based AI approach to an intelligence-based EI approach.

Keywords

Intelligence Expertise Knowledge Artificial Intelligence Cognitive modeling Expert Systems 

References

  1. 1.
    Russell, B.: Mathematical logic as based on the theory of types. Am. J. Math. 30(3), 222–262 (1908)MathSciNetzbMATHGoogle Scholar
  2. 2.
    Keravnou, E.T., Washbrook, J.: Deep and shallow models in medical expert systems. Artif. Intell. Med. 1(1), 11–28 (1989)Google Scholar
  3. 3.
    Giuse, D.A., Giuse, N.B., Miller, R.A.: Towards computer-assisted maintenance of medical knowledge bases. Artif. Intell. Med. 2(1), 21–33 (1990)Google Scholar
  4. 4.
    Baum, E.B.: Toward a model of intelligence as an economy of agents. Mach. Learn. 35(2), 155–185 (1999)zbMATHGoogle Scholar
  5. 5.
    Boegl, K., Adlassnig, K.-P., Hayashi, Y., Rothenfluh, T.E., Leitich, H.: Knowledge acquisition in the fuzzy knowledge representation framework of a medical consultation system. Artif. Intell. Med. 30(1), 1–26 (2004)Google Scholar
  6. 6.
    Reed, S.K., Pease, A.: Reasoning from imperfect knowledge. Cogn. Syst. Res. 41, 56–72 (2017)Google Scholar
  7. 7.
    Biundo, S., Bercher, P., Geier, T., Müller, F., Schattenberg, B.: Advanced user assistance based on ai planning. Cogn. Syst. Res. 12(3–4), 219–236 (2011)Google Scholar
  8. 8.
    Zhong, N., Liu, J., Sun, R.: Intelligent agents and data mining for cognitive systems. Cogn. Syst. Res. 3(5), 169–170 (2004)Google Scholar
  9. 9.
    Müller, V.C.: Introduction: philosophy and theory of artificial intelligence. Minds Mach. 22, 67–69 (2012)Google Scholar
  10. 10.
    Wang, P.: Analogy in a general-purpose reasoning system. Cogn. Syst. Res. 10(3), 286–296 (2009)Google Scholar
  11. 11.
    De Kamps, M.: Towards truly human-level intelligence in artificial applications. Cogn. Syst. Res. 14(1), 1–9 (2012)MathSciNetGoogle Scholar
  12. 12.
    Liao, S.-H.: Expert system methodologies and applicationsa decade review from 1995 to 2004. Expert Syst. Appl. 28(1), 93–103 (2005)MathSciNetGoogle Scholar
  13. 13.
    Wai, J.: What does it mean to be an expert? Intelligence 45, 122–123 (2014)Google Scholar
  14. 14.
    Meadows, A.J.: The Origins of Information Science, volume 1. Taylor Graham and the Institute of Information Scientists, London (1987)Google Scholar
  15. 15.
    Hart, A.: Knowledge acquisition for expert systems. In: Göranzon, B., Josefson, I. (eds.) Knowledge, Skill and Artificial Intelligence, pp. 103–111. Springer, Boston (1988).  https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4471-1632-5Google Scholar
  16. 16.
    Walker, A.: Knowledge systems: principles and practice. IBM J. Res. Develop. 30(1), 2–13 (1986)Google Scholar
  17. 17.
    Scott, I., Gronow, S.: Valuation expertise: its nature and application. J. Val. 8(4), 362–375 (1990)Google Scholar
  18. 18.
    Minsky, M.: A framework for representing knowledge (1974)Google Scholar
  19. 19.
    Kaur, H., Jyoti, Y.J., Arora, A., Dolly, L.A.: Artificial intelligence: bringing expert knowledge to computers. Discov. J. 2, 4–7 (2012)Google Scholar
  20. 20.
    Takashima, Q.: Characteristics and technical challenges of current expert systems. In: Proceedings of the Technology Assessment and Management Conference of the Gottlieb Duttwiler Institute, Rüschlikon, Zürich, Switzerland, pp. 31–48 (1985)Google Scholar
  21. 21.
    Mifflin, H.: The American Heritage Dictionary of the English Language. American Heritage, New York (2000)Google Scholar
  22. 22.
    Anderson, M.: Intelligence and Development: A Cognitive Theory. Blackwell Publishing, Oxford (1992)Google Scholar
  23. 23.
    Binet, A., Simon, T.: Méthodes nouvelles pour le diagnostic du niveau intellectuel des anormaux. L’année Psychologique 11(1), 191–244 (1904)Google Scholar
  24. 24.
    Bingham, W.V.: Aptitudes and Aptitude Testing. Harper, New York (1937)Google Scholar
  25. 25.
    Sternberg, R.J.: Handbook of Intelligence. Cambridge University Press, New York (2000)Google Scholar
  26. 26.
    Legg, S., Hutter, M.: A formal measure of machine intelligence. arXiv preprint cs/0605024 (2006)Google Scholar
  27. 27.
    Spearman, C.: The theory of two factors. Psychol. Rev. 21(2), 101 (1914)MathSciNetGoogle Scholar
  28. 28.
    Thurstone, L.L.: Primary Mental Abilities. University of Chicago, Chicago (1938)Google Scholar
  29. 29.
    Cattell, R.B.: Abilities: Their Structure, Growth, and Action. Houghton Mifflin, Boston (1971)Google Scholar
  30. 30.
    Howard, G.: Frames of Mind: The Theory of Multiple Intelligences. Basics, NY (1983)Google Scholar
  31. 31.
    Sternberg, R.J., Sternberg, R.J.: Beyond IQ: A Triarchic Theory of Human Intelligence. CUP Archive, Cambridge (1985)zbMATHGoogle Scholar
  32. 32.
    Eysenck, H.J.: The Structure of Human Personality (Psychology Revivals). Routledge, New York (2013)Google Scholar
  33. 33.
    Newell, A.: Heuristic programming: Ill-structured problems. In: Aronofsky, J. (ed.) Progress in Operations Research III, pp. 360–414. Wiley, New York (1969)Google Scholar
  34. 34.
    Buchanan, B., Sutherland, G., Feigenbaum, E.A.: Heuristic DENDRAL : A Program for Generating Explanatory Hypotheses in Organic Chemistry. Stanford University, Stanford (1968)zbMATHGoogle Scholar
  35. 35.
    Lindsay, R.K., Buchanan, B.G., Feigenbaum, E.A., Lederberg, J.: Dendral: a case study of the first expert system for scientific hypothesis formation. Artif. Intell. 61(2), 209–261 (1993)Google Scholar
  36. 36.
    Hoffman, R.R.: How can expertise be defined? implications of research from cognitive psychology. In: Williams, R., Faulkner, W., Fleck, J. (eds.) Exploring Expertise, pp. 81–100. Palgrave Macmillan, London (1998)Google Scholar
  37. 37.
    Feltovich, P.J., Prietula, M.J., Ericsson, K.A.: Studies of expertise from psychological perspectives. In: Ericsson, K.A., Hoffman, R., Kozbelt, A., Williams, A.M. (eds.) The Cambridge Handbook of Expertise and Expert Performance, pp. 41–67. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (2006)Google Scholar
  38. 38.
    Ericsson, K.A., Krampe, R.T., Tesch-Römer., C.: The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychol. Rev. 100(3), 363 (1993)Google Scholar
  39. 39.
    Ericsson, K.A., Lehmann, A.C.: Expert and exceptional performance: evidence of maximal adaptation to task constraints. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 47(1), 273–305 (1996)Google Scholar
  40. 40.
    Campitelli, G., Gobet, F.: Deliberate practice: necessary but not sufficient. Current Dir. Psychol. Sci. 20(5), 280–285 (2011)Google Scholar
  41. 41.
    Lesgold, A., Rubinson, H., Feltovich, P., Glaser, R., Klopfer, D., Wang, Y.: Diagnosing x-ray pictures, Expertise in a complex skill (1988)Google Scholar
  42. 42.
    Herbig, B., Glöckner, A.: Experts and decision making: first steps towards a unifying theory of decision making in novices, intermediates and experts (2009)Google Scholar
  43. 43.
    Chase, W.G., Ericsson, K.A.: Skill and working memory. Psychol. Learn. Motiv. 16, 1–58 (1982)Google Scholar
  44. 44.
    Bottou, L.: From machine learning to machine reasoning. Mach. learn. 94(2), 133–149 (2014)MathSciNetGoogle Scholar
  45. 45.
    Sternberg, R.J.: Intelligence, wisdom, and creativity: three is better than one. Educ. Psychol. 21(3), 175–190 (1986)Google Scholar
  46. 46.
    Beni, G.: From swarm intelligence to swarm robotics. In: Şahin, E., Spears, W.M. (eds.) SR 2004. LNCS, vol. 3342, pp. 1–9. Springer, Heidelberg (2005).  https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-540-30552-1_1Google Scholar
  47. 47.
    Hämäläinen, R.P., Saarinen, E.: Systems intelligence: a key competence in human action and organizational life. Systems intelligence in leadership and everyday life, pp. 39–50 (2007)Google Scholar
  48. 48.
    Grasso, A., Convertino, G.: Collective intelligence in organizations: tools and studies. Comput. Support. Coop. Work (CSCW) 21(4–5), 357–369 (2012)Google Scholar
  49. 49.
    De Groot, A.D.: Thought and Choice in Chess, vol. 4. Walter de Gruyter GmbH & Co KG, Berlin (2014)Google Scholar
  50. 50.
    Ericsson, K.A., Charness, N.: Expert performance: its structure and acquisition. Am. Psychol. 49(8), 725 (1994)Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Jabez Christopher
    • 1
  • Rajendra Prasath
    • 2
  • Odelu Vanga
    • 1
    Email author
  1. 1.Department of Computer Science and Information SystemsBITS-Pilani (Hyderabad Campus)HyderabadIndia
  2. 2.Department of Computer Science and EngineeringIndian Institute of Information Technology Sri CityChittoorIndia

Personalised recommendations