Advertisement

Events and the Origin of the World

  • Wolff-Michael Roth
Chapter
Part of the Cultural Psychology of Education book series (CPED, volume 9)

Abstract

One of the key observations in Chap.  3 is that objects, including sound-words, are social through and through. This is apparent from the fact that an object is itself a regularity of an event that intersects with those families of events that are associated with and make a living person (i.e. person-as-event). In other words, “an object is an ingredient in the character of an event” (Whitehead 1920, 143, emphasis added), where the character itself is an abstraction. We could also say that the object is social because it is a character of the unity/identity that we denote by the term {person | environment}. If an object were to exist for one person only, then this would mean that it could not be expressed in some way through an actional event and others would thus never be able to ascertain its existence. But even our most intimate, personal, perhaps even secretive thoughts are expressed in verbal and other forms to be understood by others. But why are all objects social? In Chaps.  2 and  3 it is shown that objects-as-things are abstractions from and shorthand forms for percipient events after these have come to a conclusion. How and when do these abstractions emerge – on ontogenetic and phylogenetic scales? We know, for example, that in the animal world, the organism behaves as if the object did not continue to exist after the percipient event has ended: dogs stop barking when passersby are out of sight (and no smell is apparent). Most tool-using chimpanzees fashion twigs and other things on the spot but do not carry them around, or pick them up to be used in some other site (though some chimpanzees reportedly do, carrying stones over large distances for the purpose of cracking nuts). Things and the world are as they appear in the present but do not exist beyond the current percipient event.

References

  1. Bakhtin, M. M. (1993). Toward a philosophy of the act. Austin: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
  2. Bateson, G. (1979). Mind and nature: A necessary unity. New York: E. P. Dutton.Google Scholar
  3. Bertau, M.-C. (2014). Inner form as a notion migrating from West to East: Acknowledging the Humboldtian tradition in cultural-historical psychology. In A. Yasnitsky, R. van der Veer, & M. Ferrari (Eds.), Handbook of cultural-historical theory (pp. 247–271). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Bruner, J. (1983). Child’s talk: Learning to use language. London: University of Oxford Press.Google Scholar
  5. Dewey, J. (2008). Later works vol. 10: Art as experience. Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press. (First published in 1934).Google Scholar
  6. Feuerbach, L. (1846). Sämtliche Werke. Zweiter Band. Philosophische Kritiken und Grundsätze [Complete works, vol. 2. Philosophical critiques and principles]. Leipzig: Otto Wigand.Google Scholar
  7. Held, R., & Hein, A. (1963). Movement-produced stimulation in the development of visually guided behavior. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 56, 872–876.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Husserl, E. (1940). Grundlegende Untersuchungen zum phänomenologischen Ursprung der Räumlichkeit der Natur [Foundational investigations of the phenomenological origin of the spatiality of nature]. In M. Farber (Ed.), Philosophical essays in memory of Edmund Husserl (pp. 307–325). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  9. Hutchins, E., & Johnson, C. M. (2009). Modeling the emergence of language as an embodied collective activity. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1, 523–546.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Kant, I. (1956). Werke Band II: Kritik der reinen Vernunft [Works vol. 2: Critique of pure reason]. Wiesbaden: Insel. (First published in 1787).Google Scholar
  11. Levinas, E. (1978). Autrement qu’être ou au-delà de l’essence [Otherwise than being or beyond essence]. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.Google Scholar
  12. Marx, K., & Engels, F. (1978). Werke Band 3: Die deutsche Ideologie [Works vol. 3: The German ideology]. Berlin: Dietz.Google Scholar
  13. Masataka, N. (2003). From index-finger extension to index-finger pointing: Ontogenesis of pointing in preverbal infants. In S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet (pp. 69–84). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  14. Mead, G. H. (1932). The philosophy of the present. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  15. Mead, G. H. (1938). The philosophy of the act. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  16. Mead, G. H. (1972). Mind, self, and society: From the standpoint of a social behaviorist. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  17. Merleau-Ponty, M. (1945). Phénoménologie de la perception [Phenomenology of perception]. Paris: Gallimard.Google Scholar
  18. Meshcheryakov, A. (1974). Slepoglyxonemye deti: razvitie psyxiki v processe formirovanija pobedenija [Deaf-blind children: Development of mind in the formation of behavior]. Moscow: Pedagogika.Google Scholar
  19. Piaget, J. (1970). Genetic epistemology. New York: W. W. Norton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1966). La psychologie de l’enfant [Child psychology]. Paris, France: Presses Universitaires de France.Google Scholar
  21. Ricœur, P. (1986). Du texte à l’action: Essaies d’herméneutique II [From text to action: Essays in hermeneutics, 2]. Paris: Éditions du Seuil.Google Scholar
  22. Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2006). Mirrors in the mind. Scientific American, 295(5), 54–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Schütz, A. (1932). Der sinnhafte Aufbau der sozialen Welt: Eine Einleitung in die verstehende Soziologie [The phenomenology of the social world]. Vienna: Julius Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Vygotskij, L. S. (1934). Myšlenie i reč’ : psixologičeskie issledovanija [Thinking and speaking: psychological investigations]. Moscow: Gosudarstvennoe social’noèskonomičeskoe isdatel’stvo.Google Scholar
  25. Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky, vol. 1: Problems of general psychology. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  26. Vygotsky, L. S. (1989). Concrete human psychology. Soviet Psychology, 27(2), 53–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Vygotsky, L. S. (1997a). The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky, vol. 3: Problems of the theory and history of psychology. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  28. Vygotsky, L. S. (1997b). The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky, vol. 4: The history of the development of higher mental functions. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
  29. Whitehead, A. N. (1919). An enquiry concerning the principles of natural knowledge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  30. Whitehead, A. N. (1920). The concept of nature. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  31. Whitehead, A. N. (1933). Adventures of ideas. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  32. Wittgenstein, L. (1997). Philosophical investigations/Philosophische Untersuchungen (2nd ed.). Oxford: Blackwell. (First published in 1953).Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  • Wolff-Michael Roth
    • 1
  1. 1.University of VictoriaVictoriaCanada

Personalised recommendations