Advertisement

C-Command in Discourse: Syntactic Principles Beyond the Sentence and Their Consequences for Acquisition Theory

  • Tom RoeperEmail author
Chapter
Part of the Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics book series (SITP, volume 48)

Abstract

We ask these questions: Where are parsing principles applied? Could they extend beyond sentences to Discourse? Modern proposals have proposed that Discourse structure can entail typical c-command relations. This in turn allows binding between quantifiers and pronouns. We argue that if children make the strongest, most falsifiable hypotheses first, they should seek to continue to parse input in terms of previously recognized sentence structure, linked by narrative structure as Keshet proposes for sequences like: Every candidate walked to the Dean. He took his diploma and sat down where every candidate = he. Data from the DELV test indicates that children will also allow quantifiers to be co-indexed with pronouns in separate sentences, contrary to the usual assumption that quantifier-scope is sentence bound, but consistent with Keshet’s Discourse structure. For instance, in the sequence: The man saw every boy. He played the piano, many children allow this connection. We argue that it can be seen as a natural step under a constrained notion of learnability that seeks the Strictest Interfaces possible. The CP may be the point of contact with Discourse. Early arguments that children adjoin new information to the Root in an unLabeled form can be extended to mean: attach new information to the Discourse with the same domination relations that allow c-command and binding.

References

  1. Bauke, L., & Roeper, T. (2018). How unLabeled nodes work: Morphological derivations and the subcomponents of UG operations. In L. Bauke & H. Weiss (Eds.), Labels and roots (pp. 233–262). The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
  2. Chechetto, C., & Donati, C. (2015). Re-labeling. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  3. Chomsky, N. (2013). Problems with projection. Lingua, 130, 33–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Crain, S., & Thornton, R. (1998). Investigations in universal Grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  5. deVilliers, J., & Merchant, J. (2006). Children’s acquisition ofeveryms. Smith College.Google Scholar
  6. deVilliers, J., Kotfila, J., & Roeper, T. (2017). When is recursion easier for children. Paper presented at GALA (Mallorca).Google Scholar
  7. Frazier, L. (1978). On comprehending sentences: Syntactic parsing strategies. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Connecticut.Google Scholar
  8. Frazier, L. (2013). A recycling approach to processing ellipsis. In L. Cheng & N. Cover (Eds.), Diagnosis in syntax. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  9. Heim, I. (1983). File change semantics and the familiarity theory of definiteness. In R. Bauerle, C. Schwarze & A. V. Stechow (Eds.), Meaning, use, and the interpretation of language (pp. 164–189). Berlin: de Gruyter.Google Scholar
  10. Kamp, H. (1981). A theory of truth and semantic representation. In J. Groenendijk, T. Jannsen & M. Stokhof (Eds.), Formal methods in the study of language. (pp. 277–322). Amsterdam: Mathematical Center.Google Scholar
  11. Kang, H.-K. 1999. Quantifier spreading by English and Korean children. Ms., University College, London.Google Scholar
  12. Kehler, A., Kertz, L., Rohde, H., & Elman, J. L. (2008). Coherence and coreference revisited. Journal of Semantics, 25, 1–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Keshet, E. (2008). Telescoping and scope economy. In C. B. Chang & H. J. Haynie (Eds.), Proceedings of the 26th West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics, (pp. 324–331). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.Google Scholar
  14. Koster, C. (1993). Errors in anaphora acquisition led publishers. Amsterdam: University of Amsterdam Dissertation.Google Scholar
  15. Lebeaux, D. (1988). Language acquisition and the form of the grammar. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Massachusetts.Google Scholar
  16. Lebeaux, D. (2000). Language acquisition and the form of grammar. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Pearson, B., & Roeper, T. (in prep). Quantifiers: Binding and discourse structure.Google Scholar
  18. Rizzi, L. (1997). The fine structure of the left periphery. Kluwer international handbooks of linguistics (pp. 281–337). Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar
  19. Roberts, C. (1989). Modal subordination and pronominal anaphora in discourse. Linguistics and Philosophy, 12(6), 683–721.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Romero, M., & Hardt, D. (2004). Ellipsis and the structure of discourse. Journal of Semantics, 21, 375–414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Roeper, T. (2014). Strict Interfaces and unlabeled nodes on the acquisition path: from unlabeled to labeled and minimal modular contact. Language Sciences, 46, Part B, 115–132.Google Scholar
  22. Roeper, T. (1992). From the initial state to V-2: Acquisition principles in action. In J. Meisel (Ed.), The acquisition of verb placement (pp. 333–371). Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Roeper, T., Strauss, U., & Pearson, B. (2009). Acquisition path of quantifiers: Two kinds of spreading. University of Massachusetts Occasional Papers in Linguistics.Google Scholar
  24. Seymour, H., Roeper, T., & deVilliers, J. (2005). DELV: Diagnostic evaluation of language variation. Austin, Texas: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
  25. Solan, L. (1981). Pronominal reference. Studies in theoretical psycholinguistics. Dordrecht: Kluwer.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Switzerland AG 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of LinguisticsUniversity of Massachsetts AmherstAmherstUSA

Personalised recommendations