The Social Context of Stress and Behavioral Medicine Research

Instructions, Experimenter Effects, and Social Interactions
  • David S. Krantz
  • Jeffrey Ratliff-Crain
Part of the The Springer Series in Behavioral Psychophysiology and Medicine book series (SSBP)

Abstract

In studies of stress, factors such as the context of the research situation, and subtle variations in the instructions presented can have significant effects on resulting psychological, behavioral, and physiological responses. Specifically, both laboratory and field psychophysiology studies involve procedural elements, from the obtaining of informed consent to payment for participation, that can be significant sources of error variance that remain even if the most stringent experimental controls are instituted. This chapter will address potential sources of bias in research that are associated with the social context in which studies are conducted.

Keywords

Behavioral Medicine Task Difficulty Heart Rate Response Reaction Time Task Manipulation Check 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Aronson, E., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1968). Experimentation in social psychology. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (2nd ed., pp. 1–80). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
  2. Blom, B. E., & Craighead, W. E. (1974). The effects of situational and instructional demand on indices of speech anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 83(6), 614–661.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Brod, J., Fencl, V., Heil, Z., & Jirka, J. (1959). Circulatory changes underlying blood pressure elevation during acute emotional stress in normotensive and hypertensive subjects. Clinical Science, 18, 269–279.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Carlsmith, J. M., Ellsworth, P. L., & Aronson, E. (1976). Methods of research in social psychology. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
  5. Cohen, F., Horowitz, M., Lazarus, R., Moos, R., Robins, L., Rose, R., & Rutter, M. (1982). Panel report on psychosocial assets and modifiers of stress. In G. Elliott & C. Eisdorfer (Eds.), Stress and human health (pp. 147–288). Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
  6. Contrada, R. J., Glass, D. C., Krakoff, L. R., Krantz, D. S., Kehoe, K., Isecke, W., Collins, L., & Elting, E. (1982). Effects of control over aversive stimulation and type-A behavior on cardiovascular and plasma catecholamine response. Psychophysiology, 19, 408–419.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Dembroski, T. M., MacDougall, J. M., Herd, J. A., & Shields, J. C. (1979). Effect of level of challenge on pressor and heart rate responses in type A and B subjects. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 9, 209–228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Elliott, G. R., & Eisdorfer, C. (Eds.). (1982). Stress and human health. Berlin: Springer.Google Scholar
  9. Elliott, R. (1966). Reaction time and heart rate as a function of magnitude of incentive and probability of success: A replication and extension. Journal of Experimental Research in Personality, 1, 174–178.Google Scholar
  10. Fisher, L. E., & Kotses, H. (1973). Race differences and experimenter race effect in galvanic skin response. Psychophysiology, 10, 578–582.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Fisher, L. E., & Kotses, H. (1974). Experimenter and subject sex effects in the skin conductance response. Psychophysiology, 11, 191–196.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Fowles, D. C., Fisher, A. E., & Tranel, D. T. (1982). The heart beats to reward: The effect of monetary incentive on heart rate. Psychophysiology, 19, 506–513.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Gardner, G. T. (1978). Effects of federal human-subjects regulations on data obtained in environmental Stressor research. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 628–634.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Glass, D. C., & Singer, J. E. (1972). Urban stress: Experiments on noise and social Stressors. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  15. Glass, D. C., Krakoff, L. R., Contrada, R., Hilton, W. F., Kehoe, K., Mannucci, E. G., Collins, C., Snow, B., & Elting, E. (1980). Effect of harassment and competition upon cardiovascular and plasma catecholamine responses in Type A and Type B individuals. Psychophysiology, 17(5), 453–463.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Glass, D. C., Lake, C. R., Contrada, R. J., Kehoe, K., & Erlanger, C. R. (1983). Stability of individual differences in physiological responses to stress. Health Psychology, 2, 317–341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Houston, B. K. (1983). Psychophysiological responding and the type-A behavior pattern. Journal of Research in Personality, 17, 22–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Kidd, R. F. (1976). Manipulation checks: Advantage or disadvantage. Representative Research in Social Psychology, 7, 160–165.Google Scholar
  19. Krantz, D. S., & Manuck, S. B. (1984). Reactivity and cardiovascular disease. Psychological Bulletin, 96, 435–464.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Krantz, D. S., Manuck, S. B., & Wing, R. (1986). Psychological Stressors and task variables as elicitors of reactivity. In K. A. Matthews, S. M. Weiss, T. Detre, T. M. Dembroski, B. Falkner, S. B. Manuck, & R. B. Williams (Eds.), Handbook of stress, reactivity, and cardiovascular disease. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  21. Lazarus, R. S. (1966). Psychological stress and the coping process. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
  22. Light, K. C., & Obrist, P. A. (1983). Task difficulty, heart rate, and cardiovascular responses to an appetitive reaction time task. Psychophysiology, 20, 301–312.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. London, M. D., & Schwartz, G. E. (1980). The interaction of instruction components with cybernetic feedback effects in the voluntary control of human heart rate. Psychophysiology, 17, 437–443.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. MacDougall, J. M., Dembroski, T. M., Slaats, S., Herd, J. A., & Eliot, R. S. (1983). Selective cardiovascular effects of stress and cigarette-smoking. Journal of Human Stress, 9(3), 13–21.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Manuck, S. B., Corse, C. D., & Winkelmen, P. A. (1979). Behavioral correlates of individual differences in blood pressure reactivity. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 23, 281–288.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Mason, J. (1971). Re-evaluation of the concept of nonspecificity in stress theory. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 8, 323–333.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. McGuigan, F. J. (1963). The experimenter: A neglected stimulus object. Psychological Bulletin, 60, 421–428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Milgram, S. (1963). Behavioral study of obedience. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67, 371–378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Murphy, J. K., Alpert, B. S., & Moes, D. M. (1985). Race and reactivity: A neglected relationship. Psychosomatic Medicine, 47(1), 91.Google Scholar
  30. Obrist, P. A., Gaebelein, C. J., Teller, E. S., Langer, A. W., Grignolo, A., Light, K. C., & McCubbin, J. A. (1978). The relationship among heart rate, carotid dP/dt and blood pressure in humans as a function of the type of stress. Psychophysiology, 15, 102–115.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Orne, M. (1962). On the social psychology of the psychological experiment. American Psychologist, 17, 776–783.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Riecken, H. W. (1962). A program for research on experiments in social psychology. In N. Washburne (Ed.), Decision, values, and groups (Vol. 2, pp. 25–41). Elmsford, NY: Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
  33. Roethlisberger, F. J., & Dickson, W. J. (1939). Management and the worker. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  34. Rosenberg, M. J. (1965). When dissonance fails: On eliminating evaluation apprehension from attitude measurement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1, 28–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Rosenthal, R. (1966). Experimenter effects in behavioral research. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  36. Rosenthal, R., & Fode, K. L. (1963). The effect of experimenter bias on the performance of the albino rat. Behavioral Science, 8, 183–189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Rosenthal, R., Persinger, G. W., Vikan Kline, L., & Mulry, R. C. (1963). The role of the research assistant in the mediation of experimenter bias. Journal of Personality, 31, 313–335.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Seligman, M. E. (1972). Learned helplessness. Annual Review of Medicine, 23, 407.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Silvia, C., Street, S., & Baum, A. (1986). Anticipation and certainty: Mediators of responding to a stressful event. Unpublished manuscript, Uniformed Services University of the Health Sciences.Google Scholar
  40. Smith, M. B. (1976). Some perspectives on ethical-political issues in social science research. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 2(4), 445–453.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Sternbach, R. A. (1964). The effects of instructional sets on autonomic responses. Psychophysiology, 1, 67–72.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Van Egeren, L. F. (1979). Cardiovascular changes during social competition in a mixed motive game. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 858–864.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Williams, R. B. (1985). Neuroendocrine response patterns and stress: Biobehavioral mechanisms of disease. In R. B. Williams (Ed.), Perspectives on behavioral medicine: Vol. 2. Neuroendocrine control and behavior (pp. 71–101). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  44. Wilson, E. B. (1952). An introduction to scientific research. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1989

Authors and Affiliations

  • David S. Krantz
    • 1
  • Jeffrey Ratliff-Crain
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Medical PsychologyUniformed Services University of the Health SciencesBethesdaUSA

Personalised recommendations