The Development of Prosocial Behavior

  • Carolyn Zahn-Waxler
  • K. Danielle Smith
Part of the Perspectives in Developmental Psychology book series (PDPS)

Abstract

There have been energetic debates throughout time regarding the socio-moral nature of human interaction. Centuries ago, theologians and philosophers framed the issues in terms of stark contrasts (e.g., humans were born with the capacity for good or evil, and these qualities were either innate or learned). Depending upon one’s point of view, societal influences could be seen either as corrupting innocent youth or as a means by which aggressive instincts could be subdued over time to produce caring and compassion. Such unidimensional views gradually began to give way, with increased recognition of our multifaceted nature. Research in the last half century has significantly advanced knowledge about biological and environmental contributors to prosocial and antisocial behaviors. Here we review research on prosocial behavior, from a developmental perspective, emphasizing mainly work published in the last decade. By prosocial or altruistic behaviors we mean acts that include provision of comfort or sympathy, helping, sharing, cooperation, rescue, protection, and defense. We also review work on emotions and cognitions that often accompany, and are integral features of, prosocial behaviors. This includes children’s empathy or emotional incorporation of the others’ emotional experience, as well as their moral reasoning and cognitive comprehension of others’ internal states and needs (perspective taking, role taking).

Keywords

Antisocial Behavior Prosocial Behavior Moral Reasoning Perspective Taking Empathic Concern 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Amsterdam, B. (1972). Mirror self-image reactions before age two. Developmental Psychology, 297-305.Google Scholar
  2. Arbuthnot, J., & Gordon, D. A. (1986). Behavioral and cognitive effects of a moral reasoning development intervention for high-risk behavior-disordered adolescents. Journal of Consulting Clinical Psychology, 54, 208–216.Google Scholar
  3. Barnett, M. A. (1987). Empathy and related responses in children. In N. Eisenberg & J. Strayer (Eds.), Empathy and its development (pp. 146–162). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  4. Barnett, M. A., & McCoy, S. A. (1989). The relation of distressful childhood experiences and empathy in college undergraduates. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 150, 417–426.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Barnett, M. A., McMinimy, V., Flouer, G., & Masbad, I. (1987). Adolescent’s evaluations of peers’ motives for helping. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 16, 579–586.Google Scholar
  6. Batson, C. D., Batson, J. G., Griffitt, C. A., Barrientos, S., Brandt, J. R., Spnengelmeyer, P., & Bayly, M. J. (1989). Negative-state relief and the empathy-altruism hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 922–933.Google Scholar
  7. Batson, C. D., Dyck, J. L., Brandt, J. R., Batson, J. G., Powell, A. L., McMaster, M. R., & Griffitt, C. (1988). Fives studies testing two new egoistic alternatives to the empathy-altruism hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 52–77.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  8. Bauman, D. J., Cialdini, R. B., & Kendrick, D. J. (1983). Mood and sex differences in the development of altruism as hedonism. Academic Psychology Bulletin, 5, 299–307.Google Scholar
  9. Baumrind, D. (1986). Sex differences in moral reasoning: Response to Walker’s (1984) conclusion that there are none. Child Development, 57, 511–521.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Bear, G. G. (1989). Sociomoral reasoning and antisocial behaviors around normal sixth graders. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 35, 181–196.Google Scholar
  11. Beardslee, W., & Podorefsky, D. (1988). Resilient adolescents whose parents have serious affective and other psychiatric disorders: Importance of self-understanding and relationships. American Journal of Psychiatry, 145(1), 63–69.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Beardslee, W. R., Schultz, L. H., & Selman, R. L. (1987). Level of social-cognitive development, adoptive functioning, and DSM-III diagnoses in adolescent offspring of parents with affective disorders: Implications for the development of the capacity for mutuality. Developmental Psychology, 23(6), 807–815.Google Scholar
  13. Bengtsson, H., & Johnson, L. (1987). Cognitions related to empathy in five-to eleven-year-old children. Child Development, 58, 1001–1012.Google Scholar
  14. Berndt, T. J. (1981). Effects of friendship on prosocial intentions and behavior. Child Development, 52, 636–643.Google Scholar
  15. Berthenthal, B., & Fischer, K. W. (1978). The development of self-recognition in the infant. Developmental Psychology, 14, 44–50.Google Scholar
  16. Blasi, A. (1980). Bridging moral cognition and moral action: A critical review of the literature. Psychology Bulletin, 88, 1–45.Google Scholar
  17. Block, J., Olweus, D., & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1986). Development of anti-social and prosocial behavior: Research, theories and issues. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  18. Bloom, L., Lightbown, P., & Hood, L. (1975). Structure and variation in child language. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, serial No. 160, Vol. 40, No. 2. Chicago, University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  19. Boehnke, K., Silbereisen, R. K., Eisenberg, N., Reykowski, J., & Polomari, A. (1989). Developmental pattern of prosocial motivation: A cross-national study. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 20, 219–243.Google Scholar
  20. Boom, J., & Molenaar, P C. (1989). A developmental model of hierarchical stage structure in objective moral judgements. Developmental Review, 9, 133–145.Google Scholar
  21. Bretherton, I., Fritz, J., Zahn-Waxler, C., & Ridgeway, D. (1986). The acquisition and development of emotion language: A functionalist perspective. Child Development, 57, 529–548.Google Scholar
  22. Brody, L. R. (1985). Gender differences in emotional development: A review of theories and research. Journal of Personality, 53, 102–149.Google Scholar
  23. Brothers, L. (1989). A biological perspective on empathy. American Journal of Psychiatry, 146, 10–19.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Bruner, J. S. (1972). The nature and uses of immaturity. American Psychologist, 27, 1–22.Google Scholar
  25. Bryan, T., Werner, M., & Pearl, R. (1982). Learning disabled students’ conformity responses to prosocial and antisocial situations. Learning Disability Quarterly, 2(1), 33–38.Google Scholar
  26. Bussey, K., & Maugham, B. (1982). Gender differences in moral reasoning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 701–706.Google Scholar
  27. Butterworth, G. (1980). A discussion of some issues raised by Piaget’s concept of childhood egocentrism. In M. V. Cox (Ed.), Are young children egocentric? pp. 17–40. York, England: Batsford Academic and Educational.Google Scholar
  28. Campos, J. J., Barrett, K. C., Lamb, M. E., Goldsmith, H. H., & Stenberg, C. (1983). Sodoemotional Development. In P H. Mussen, M. M. Haith, & J. J. Campos (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Infancy and developmental psychobiology (Vol. 2, pp. 783–915). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  29. Caplan, M. & Hay, D. E (1989). Preschoolers’ responses to peers’ distress and beliefs about bystander intervention. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 231–242.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  30. Carlson, M., & Miller, N. (1987). Explanation of the relation between negative mood and helping. Psychological Bulletin, 102(1), 191–208.Google Scholar
  31. Chambers, J. H., & Ascione, F. R. (1987). The effects of prosocial and aggressive videogames on children’s donating and helping. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 148, 499–505.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  32. Chandler, M. J. (1973). Egocentric and anti-social behavior: The assessment and training of social perspective-taking skills. Developmental Psychology, 9(3), 326–332.Google Scholar
  33. Chandler, M. J., Greenspan, S., & Barenboim, C. (1974). Assessment and training of role-taking skills in institutionalized emotionally disturbed children. Developmental Psychology, 10(4), 546–553.Google Scholar
  34. Chapman, M., Zahn-Waxler, C., Cooperman, G., & Iannotti, R. (1987). Empathy and responsibility in the motivation of children’s helping. Developmental Psychology, 23, 140–145.Google Scholar
  35. Cialdini, R. B., Eisenberg, N., Shell, R., & McCreath, H. (1987). Commitments to help by children: Effects on subsequent prosocial self-attributions. British Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 237–245.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Cialdini, R. B., & Kendrick, D. T. (1976). Altruism as hedonism: A social development perspective on the relationship of negative mood and state and helping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 907–914.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Cialdini, R. B., Schaller, M., Houlihan, D, Arps, K., Fultz, J., & Beaman, A. L. (1987). Empathy-based helping: Is it selflessly motivated? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 749–758.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Cicchetti, D. (1990). The organization and coherence of socioemotional, cognitive and representational development: Illustrations through a developmental psychopathology perspective on down syndrome and child maltreatment. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1988: Socioemotional Development, R. Thompson, (Ed.), Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, pp. 259–366.Google Scholar
  39. Clary, E. G., & Miller, J. (1986). Socialization and situational influences on sustained altruism. Child Development, 57, 1358–1369.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  40. Cummings, E. M., Hollenbeck, B., Iannotti, R. J., Radke-Yarrow, M., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (1986). Early organization of altruism and aggression: Developmental patterns and individual differences. In C. Zahn-Waxler, E. M. Cummings, & R. J. Iannotti (Eds.), Altruism and aggression: Biological and social origins (pp. 165–188) New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  41. Cummings, M., Zahn-Waxler, C., & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1981). Young children’s responses to expressions of anger and affection by others in the family. Child Development, 52, 1274–1282.Google Scholar
  42. Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of the emotions in man and animal. London: John Murray.Google Scholar
  43. Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  44. Denham, S. A. (1986). Social cognition, prosocial behavior and emotion in preschoolers: Contextual validation. Child Development, 57, 194–201.Google Scholar
  45. Denham, S. A., & Couchard, E. A. (1991). Social-emotional predictors of preschoolers’ responses to adult negative emotion. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 32, 595–608.Google Scholar
  46. Denham, S., McKinley, M., Couchard, E. A., & Holt, R. (1991). Emotional and behavioral predictors of preschool peer ratings. Child Development, 61, 1145–1152.Google Scholar
  47. Derr, A. M. (1986). How learning disabled adolescent boys make moral judgments. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 19, 160–164.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  48. Donenberg, G. R., & Hoffman, L. W. (1988). Gender differences in moral development. Sex Roles, 18, 701–717.Google Scholar
  49. Dubow, E. E., Huesmann, L. R., & Eron, L. D. (1987). Mitigating aggression and promoting prosocial behavior in aggressive elementary schoolboys. Behavior Research Theories, 25, 527–531.Google Scholar
  50. Dunn, J., & Munn, P. (1986). Siblings and the development of prosocial behavior. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 9, 265–284.Google Scholar
  51. Eagly, A. H., & Crowley, M. (1986). Gender and helping behavior: A meta-analytic review of the social psychological literature. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 283–308.Google Scholar
  52. Eckerman, C. O., Whatley, J. L., & Kutz, S. L. (1975). Growth of social play with peers during the second year of life. Developmental Psychology, 11, 42–49.Google Scholar
  53. Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Bustamante, D., Mathy, R. M., Miller, P. A., & Lindholm, E. (1988). Differentiation of vicariously induced emotional reactions in children. Developmental Psychology, 24, 237–246.Google Scholar
  54. Eisenberg, N., Fabes, P. A., Miller, P. A., Fultz, J., Shell, R., Mathy, R. M., & Reno, R. R. (1989). Relation of sympathy and personal distress to prosocial behavior: A multimethod study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 55–66.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  55. Eisenberg, N., & Lennon, R. (1983). Sex differences in empathy and related capacities. Psychological Bulletin, 94, 100–131.Google Scholar
  56. Eisenberg, N., & Miller, P. A. (1987). The relation of empathy to prosocial and related behaviors. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 91–119.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Eisenberg, N., Schaller, M., Fabes, R. A., Bustamante, D., Mathy, R. M., Shell, R., & Rhodes, K. (1988). Differentiation of personal distress and sympathy in children and adults. Developmental Psychology, 24, 766–775.Google Scholar
  58. Eisenberg, N., Shell, R., Pasternack, J., Lennon, R., Belber, R., & Matty, R. M. (1987). Prosocial development in middle childhood: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 23, 712–718.Google Scholar
  59. Eisenberg, N., & Strayer, J. (1987). Empathy and its development. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  60. Emde, R., Johnson, W., & Easterbrooks, M. A. (1987). The do’s and don’ts of early moral development: Psychoanalytic tradition and current research. In J. Kagan & S. Lamb (Eds.), The emergence of morality in young children (pp. 245–276). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  61. Emery, R. E. (1982). Interparent conflict and the children of discord and divorce. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 310–330.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Fabes, R. A., Eisenberg, N., & Miller, P. (1990). Maternal correlates of children’s vicarious emotional responsiveness. Developmental Psychology, 26, 639–648.Google Scholar
  63. Fabes, R. A., Fultz, J., Eisenberg, N., May-Plumlee, T, & Christopher, F. S. (1989). Effects of rewards on children’s prosocial motivation: A socialization study. Developmental Psychology, 25, 509–515.Google Scholar
  64. Fergusion, T. J., & Rule, B. G. (1988). Children’s evaluation of retaliatory aggression. Child Development, 59, 961–968.Google Scholar
  65. Feshbach, N. D. (1978). Studies of empathie behavior in children. In B. A. Maher (Ed.), Progress in experimental personality research (Vol. 8, pp. 1–47), New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  66. Feshbach, N. D. (1979). Empathy training: A field study in affective education. In S. Feshbach & A. Fraczek (Eds.), Aggression and behavior change: Biological and social processes (pp. 234–249). New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
  67. Field, T., Woodson, R., Greenberg, R., & Cohen, D. (1982). Discrimination and imitation of facial expressions by neonates. Science, 218, 179–181.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  68. Francis, L. J., & Pearson, P. R. (1987). Empathic development during adolescence: Religiosity, the missing link? Personality and Individual Differences, 8, 145–148.Google Scholar
  69. Freud, S. (1958). Civilization and its discontents. New York: Doubleday Anchor Books.Google Scholar
  70. Frodi, A., Lamb, M. E., Leavitt, L. A., Donovan, W. A., Neff, C., & Sherry, D. (1978). Fathers’ and mothers’ responses to the faces and cries of normal and premature infants. Developmental Psychology, 14(5), 490–498.Google Scholar
  71. Frodi, A., Macaulay, J., & Thome, P. R. (1977). Are women always less aggressive than men? A review of the experimental literature. Psychological Bulletin, 84(4), 634–660.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Gilligan, C. (1977). In a different voice: Women’s conception of self and morality. Harvard Educational Review, 47, 481–517.Google Scholar
  73. Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: Psychological theory and women’s development. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
  74. Grusec, J. E. (1982). The socialization of altruism. In N. Eisenberg-Berg (Ed.), The development of prosocial behavior (pp. 139–166). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  75. Grusec, J. E. (1991). Socializing concern for others in the home. Developmental Psychology, 27(2), 338–342.Google Scholar
  76. Hall, J. A. (1978). Gender differences in decoding nonverbal cues. Psychological Bulletin, 85(4), 845–857.Google Scholar
  77. Hart, D. (1988). A longitudinal study of adolescents’ socialization and identification as predictors of adult moral judgment development. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 34, 245–260.Google Scholar
  78. Hartshorne, H., May, M. A., & Mahler, J. B. (1929). Studies in the nature of character (Vol. 2), Studies in service and self-control. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  79. Haviland, J. M., & Lelivica, M. (1987). The induced affect response: 10-week-old infants’ responses to three emotion expressions. Developmental Psychology, 23(1), 97–104.Google Scholar
  80. Haynes-Seman, C. (1987). Developmental origins of moral masochism: A failure-to-thrive toddler’s interactions with mother. Child Abuse and Neglect, 11, 319–330.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  81. Hinshaw, S. P., Henker, B., Whalen, C. K., Erhardt, D, & Dunnighton, R. E. (1989). Aggressive, prosocial, and nonsocial behavior in hyperactive boys: Dose effects of methylphenidate in naturalistic settings. Journal of Consulting Clinical Psychology, 57, 636–643.Google Scholar
  82. Hobson, R. R (1984). Early childhood autism and the question of egocentrism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 14, 85–104.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  83. Hoffman, M. L. (1970). Moral development. In P H. Mussen (Ed.), Carmichael’s manual of child psychology: (Vol. 2) (pp. 261–360). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  84. Hoffman, M. L. (1975). Developmental synthesis of affect and cognition and its implications for altruistic motivation. Developmental Psychology, 11, 605–622.Google Scholar
  85. Hoffman, M. L. (1977). Sex differences in empathy and related behaviors. Psychological Bulletin, 84, 712–722.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  86. Howes, C., & Eldridge, R. (1985). Responses of abused, neglected and non-maltreated children to the behaviors of their peers. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 6, 261–270.Google Scholar
  87. Howes, C., & Farver, J. (1987). Toddler’s responses to the distress of their peers. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 8, 441–452.Google Scholar
  88. Hudgins, W, & Prentice, N. M. (1973). Moral judgment in delinquent and non-delinquent adolescents and their mothers. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 82, 145–152.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  89. Izard, C. E. (1977). Human emotions. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
  90. Jurkovic, G. J. (1980). The juvenile delinquent as a moral philosopher: A structural-developmental perspective. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 709–727.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  91. Kagan, J., & Lamb, S. (1987). The emergence of morality in young children. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  92. Kaplan, P. J., & Arbuthnot, J. (1985). Affective empathy and cognitive role-taking in delinquent and nondeliquent youth. Adolescence, 20, 323–333.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  93. Keltikangus-Tarvinen, L. (1989). Moral judgements of aggressive and non-aggressive children. The Journal of Social Psychology, 129, 733–739.Google Scholar
  94. Klimes-Dougan, B., & Kistner, J. (1990). Physically abused preschoolers’ responses to peers’ distress. Developmental Psychology, 26(4), 599–602.Google Scholar
  95. Koestner, R., Franz, C., & Weinberger, J. (1990). The family origins of empathie concern: A 26-year longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 709–717.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  96. Kohlberg, L. (1969). Stage and sequence: The cognitive-developmental approach to socialization. In D. A. Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and research (pp. 347–480). New York: Rand McNally.Google Scholar
  97. Lee, M., & Prentice, N. M. (1988). Interrelations of empathy, cognition, and moral reasoning with dimensions of juvenile delinquency. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 16, 127–139.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  98. Lennon, R., & Eisenberg, N. (1987). Emotional displays associated with preschoolers’ prosocial behavior. Child Development, 58, 992–1000.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  99. Lewis, M., Sullivan, M., Stanger, C., & Weiss, M. (1989). Self development and self-conscious emotions. Child Development, 60, 146–156.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  100. MacLean, P. D. (1985). Brain evolution relating to family, play, and the separation call. Archives of General Psychology, 42, 405–417.Google Scholar
  101. Main, M., & George, C. (1985). Response of abused and disadvantaged toddlers to distress in playmates: A study in the day care setting. Developmental Psychology, 21, 407–412.Google Scholar
  102. Main, M., & Goldwyn, R. (1984). Predicting rejection of her infant from mother’s representation of her own experience: Implications for the abused-abusing intergenerational cycle. Child Abuse and Neglect, 8, 203–217.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  103. Matthews, K. A., Batson, C. D., Horn, J., & Rosenman, R. H. (1981). Principles in his nature which interest him in the fortune of others: The heritability of empathie concern for others. Journal of Personality, 49, 237–247.Google Scholar
  104. McCall, R. B. (1979). Qualitative transitions in behavioral development in the first two years. In M. H. Bornstein & W. Kessen (Eds.), Psychological development in infancy (pp. 182–224). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  105. McCord, L. (1990). Altruism, mutual aid and aggression. Paper presentation at workshop, hostility and sociability: Developmental and socio-psychological perspectives, Warsaw, Poland.Google Scholar
  106. Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 75–78.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  107. Miller, P. A., & Eisenberg, N. (1988). The relation of empathy to aggressive and externalizing/antisocial behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 324–344.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  108. Mills, R. S. L., & Grusec, J. E. (1989). Cognitive, affective, and behavioral consequences of praising altruism. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 35, 299–326.Google Scholar
  109. Muuss, R. E. (1988). Carol Gilligan’s theory of sex differences in the development of moral reasoning during adolescence. Adolescence, 23, 229–243.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  110. Nelms, B. C. (1989). Emotional behaviors in chronically ill children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 17, 657–668.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  111. Nicholich, L. (1977). Beyond sensorimotor intelligence: Assessment of symbolic maturity through analyses of pretend play. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 23, 89–99.Google Scholar
  112. Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1987). Sex differences in unipolar depression: Evidence and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 101(2), 257–282.Google Scholar
  113. Olweus, D., Block, J., & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1986). Development of antisocial and prosocial behavior. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  114. Parke, R. D., & Slaby, R. G. (1983). The development of aggression. In P. H. Mussen & E. M. Hetherington (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Socialization, personality, and social development (Vol. 4, pp. 547–641). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  115. Parmelee, A. (1986). Children’s illnesses: Their beneficial effects on behavioral development. Child Development, 57, 1–10.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  116. Pecukis, E. V. (1990). A cognitive/affective empathy training program as a function of ego development in aggressive adolescent females. Adolescence, 25, 59–76.Google Scholar
  117. Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
  118. Piaget, J. (1965). The moral judgement of the child. New York: Harcourt, Brace. (Originally published 1932).Google Scholar
  119. Radke-Yarrow, M., Zahn-Waxier, C., & Chapman, M. (1983). Children’s prosocial dispositions and behavior. In P. H. Mussen & E. M. Hetherington (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Socialization, personality, and social development (Vol. 4, pp. 469–545). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  120. Redefer, L. A., & Goodman, J. F. (1989). Pet-facilitated therapy with autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 461–467.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  121. Rehberg, H. R., & Richman, C. L. (1989). Prosocial behavior in preschool children: A look at the interaction of race, gender, and family composition. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 12, 385–401.Google Scholar
  122. Renshaw, P. D., & Asher, S. R. (1983). Children’s goals and strategies for social interaction. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 29, 353–374.Google Scholar
  123. Rheingold, H. L., & Emery, G. N. (1986). The nurturant acts of very young children. In D. Olweus, J. Block, & M. Radke-Yarrow (Eds.), The development of anti-and prosocial behavior (pp. 75–96). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  124. Rheingold, H. L., Hay, D. F., & West, M. J. (1976). Sharing in the second year of life. Child Development, 47, 1148–1158.Google Scholar
  125. Robins, L. N. (1986). The consequences of conduct disorder in girls. In D. Olweus, J. Block, & M. Radke-Yarrow (Eds.), Development of antisocial and prosocial behavior: Research, theories and issues (pp. 385–414). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  126. Robinson, J. (1989). Sex differences in the development of empathy during late infancy: Findings from the MacArthur longitudinal twin study. Presentation at SRCD, Kansas City, Missouri.Google Scholar
  127. Rosenhan, D. L. (1970). The natural socialization of altruistic autonomy. In J. R. Macaulay & L. Berkowitz (Eds.), Altruism and helping behavior (pp. 251–268). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  128. Ross, H. S., & Goldman, B. D. (1977). Establishing new social relations in infancy. In T. Alloway, L. Kramer, & P. Pliner (Eds.), Advances in the study of communication and affect, Attachment behavior (Vol. 3, pp. 61–79). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
  129. Rushton, J. P. (1976). Socialization and the altruistic behavior of children, Psychological Bulletin, 83, 898–913.Google Scholar
  130. Rushton, J. P., Fulker, D. W., Neale, M. C., Nias, D. L. B., & Eysenck, H. J. (1986). Altruism and aggression: The heritability of individual differences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 1192–1198.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  131. Sagi, A., & Hoffman, M. L. (1976). Empathic distress in the newborn. Developmental Psychology, 12, 175–176.Google Scholar
  132. Sears, R. R., Maccoby, E. E., & Levin, H. (1957). Patterns of child rearing. Evanston IL: Row and Peterson.Google Scholar
  133. Segal, N. L. (1984). Cooperation, competition, and altruism within twin sets: A reappraisal. Ethology and Sociobiology, 5, 163–177.Google Scholar
  134. Segal, N. L. (1988). Cooperation, competition, and altruism in human twinships: A sociobiological approach. In K. B. MacDonald (Ed.), Sociobiological perspectives on human development (pp. 168–206). New York: Springer-Verlag.Google Scholar
  135. Shigetomi, C. C., Hartmann, D. P., & Gelfand, D. M. (1981). Sex differences in children’s altruistic behavior and reputations for helpfulness. Developmental Psychology, 17, 434–437.Google Scholar
  136. Siegal, M. (1985). Mother-child relations and the development of empathy: A short-term longitudinal study. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 57, 355–365.Google Scholar
  137. Sigman, M., Ungerer, J. A., & Russell, A. (1983). Moral judgements in relation to behavioral and cognitive disorders in adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 11, 503–511.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  138. Simner, M. L. (1971). Newborn’s response to the cry of another infant. Developmental Psychology, 5, 136–150.Google Scholar
  139. Stanhope, L., Bell, R. Q., & Parker-Cohen, N. Y. (1987). Temperament and helping behavior in preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 23, 347–353.Google Scholar
  140. Stern, D. (1985). The interpersonal world of the “infant”. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  141. Strain, P., & Odom, S. L. (1986). Peer social initiations: Effective intervention for social skills development of exceptional children. Exceptional Children, 56, 543–551.Google Scholar
  142. Strayer, J., & Roberts, W. (1989). Children’s empathy and role taking: Child and parental factors, and relations to prosocial behavior. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 10, 227–239.Google Scholar
  143. Termine, N. T., & Izard, C. E. (1988). Infants’ responses to their mothers’ expressions of joy and sadness. Developmental Psychology, 24, 223–229.Google Scholar
  144. Teti, D. M., & Ablard, K. A. (1989). Security of attachment and infant-sibling relationships: A laboratory study. Child Development, 60, 1519–1528.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  145. Thompson, R. A., & Hoffman, M. L. (1980). Empathy and the development of guilt in children. Developmental Psychology, 16, 155–156.Google Scholar
  146. Trevarthen, C. (1989, Autumn). Origins and directions for the concept of infant intersubjectivity. Society for Research in Child Development. Newsletter, pp. 1-4.Google Scholar
  147. Walker, L. J. (1984). Sex differences in the development of moral reasoning: A critical review. Child Development, 55, 677–691.Google Scholar
  148. Watson, M., & Fischer, K. (1977). A developmental sequence of agent use in late infancy. Child Development, 48, 828–836.Google Scholar
  149. Wiesenfeld, A. R., Whitman, P B., & Malatesta, C. Z. (1984). Individual differences among adult women in sensitivity to infants: Evidence in support of an empathy concept. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 118–124.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  150. Yarrow, M. R., Scott, P. M., & Waxier, C. Z. (1973). Learning concern for others. Developmental Psychology, 8, 240–260.Google Scholar
  151. Zahn-Waxler, C., Cole, P., & Barrett, K. C. (1991). Guilt and empathy: Sex differences and implications for the development of depression. In K. Dodge & J. Garber (Eds.), Emotion regulation and dysregulation (pp. 243–272). New York: Cambridge Press.Google Scholar
  152. Zahn-Waxler, C., Cummings, E. M., & Iannotti, R. (1986). Altruism and aggression: Biological and social origins. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  153. Zahn-Waxler, C., Friedman, S. L., & Cummings, E. M. (1983). Children’s emotions and behaviors in response to infants’ cries. Child Development, 54, 1522–1528.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  154. Zahn-Waxler, C., & Kochanska, G. (1990). The development of guilt. In R. Thompson (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 1988: Socioemotional Development (pp. 183–258). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
  155. Zahn-Waxler, C., Kochanska, G., Krupnick, J., & McKnew, D. (1990). Patterns of guilt in children of depressed and well mothers. Developmental Psychology, 26(1), 51–59.Google Scholar
  156. Zahn-Waxler, C., & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1982). The development of altruism: Alternative research strategies. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), The development of prosocial behavior (pp. 109–137). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  157. Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., & King, R. (1979). Child rearing and children’s prosocial initiations toward victims of distress. Child Development, 50, 319–330.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  158. Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., Chapman, M., & Wagner, E. (1990). The development of concern for others. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
  159. Zahn-Waxler, C., Robinson, J., & Emde, R. (1990). The development and heritability of empathy. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
  160. Zarbatany, L., Hartmann, D. P., & Gelfand, D. M. (1985). Why does children’s generosity increase with age: Susceptibility to experimenter influence or altruism? Child Development, 56, 746–756.PubMedGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1992

Authors and Affiliations

  • Carolyn Zahn-Waxler
    • 1
  • K. Danielle Smith
    • 1
  1. 1.Laboratory of Developmental PsychologyNational Institute of Mental HealthBethesdaUSA

Personalised recommendations