Handedness and Lateralization

  • Polly Henninger
Part of the Critical Issues in Neuropsychology book series (CINP)

Abstract

To accurately assess the nature and extent of brain injury, is it important to know the patient’s handedness? Evidence from both clinical and normal populations indicates that the organization of brain functions differs for right- and left-handers. In general, left-handers are less lateralized for linguistic, visuospatial, and other cognitive and affective processes, and are likely to show bilateral representation of cognitive functions. Lack of standardization in the measurement of handedness and the influence of other variables such as familial sinistrality, sex, and reasoning ability on brain organization make it difficult to specify for any particular individual. However, awareness of the identified modifiers of brain organization and knowledge of findings on handedness relating to lateralized functions can assist the neuropsychologist in making more accurate diagnoses and inferences. This chapter will present current research findings on laterality and handedness and apply them to issues of clinical concern.

Keywords

Left Hemisphere Spatial Ability Hand Preference Dichotic Listening Hemispheric Specialization 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Annett, M. (1970). A classification of hand preference by association analysis. British Journal of Psychology, 61, 303–321.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Annett, M. (1972). The distribution of manual asymmetry. British Journal of Psychology, 63, 343–358.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Annett, M. (1973). Handedness in families. Annals of Human Genetics, 37, 93–105.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Annett, M. (1975). Hand preference and the laterality of cerebral speech. Cortex, 11, 305–328.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Annett, M. (1985). Left, right, hand and brain: The right shift theory. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  6. Ardila, A., Correa, P., Zuluaga, J., & Uribe, B. (1988). Spatial abilities in “forced” left-handers. Developmental Neuropsychology, 4(2), 147–150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bakan, P. (1971). Handedness and birth order. Nature, 229, 195.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Bakan, P. (1973). Left-handedness & alcoholism. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 36, 514.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Bates, E., O’Connell, B., Vaid, J., Sledge, P., & Oakes, S. (1986). Language and hand preference in early development. Developmental Neuropsychology, 2(1), 1–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Benton, A. L. (1965). The problem of cerebral dominance. The Canadian Psychologist, 6a(4), 332–348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Bishop, D. V M. (1980). Handedness, clumsiness and cognitive ability. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 22, 569–579.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Blau, A. (1946). The master hand. New York: American Ortho-Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
  13. Borod, J. C., Koff, E., & Caron, H. S. (1984). The Target Test: A brief laterality measure of speed and accuracy. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 58, 743–748.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Borod, J. C., Carper, M., Naeser, M., & Goodglass, H. (1985). Left-handed and right-handed aphasics with left hemisphere lesions compared on nonverbal performance measures. Cortex, 21, 81–90.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Briggs, G. G., & Nebes, R. D. (1976). The effects of handedness, family history and sex on the performance of a dichotic listening task. Neuropsychologia, 14, 129–133.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Bryden, M. P. (1988). An overview of the dichotic listening procedure and its relation to cerebral organization. In K. Jugdahl (Ed.), Handbook of dichotic listening (pp. 1–44). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  17. Bryden, M. P, & MacDonald, V. (1987). The measurement of handedness and its relation to neuropsychological issues. Unpublished manuscript, University of Waterloo.Google Scholar
  18. Bryden, M. P, Hecaen, H., & DeAgostini, M. (1983). Patterns of cerebral organization. Brain and Language, 20, 249–262.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Casey, M. B., Brabeck, M. M., & Ludlow, L. H. (1986). Familial handedness and its relation to spatial ability following strategy instructions. Intelligence, 10, 389–406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Chapman, L. J., & Chapman, J. R (1987). The measurement of handedness. Brain and Cognition, 6, 175–183.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Cohen, D. B. (1978). Dark hair and light eyes in college students: A potential biologic marker for liability to psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 57 (4), 455–458.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Corballis, M. C. (1980). Laterality and myth. American Psychologist, 35(3), 284–295.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Curry, F. K. W, & Rutherford, D. R. (1967). Recognition and recall of dichotically presented verbal stimuli by right-and left-handed persons. Neuropsychologia, 5, 119–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. DAmico, C. M., & Kimura, D. (1987). Evidence for subgroups of adextrals based on speech lateralization and cognitive patterns (Research Bulletin 664). London, Canada: University of Western Ontario, Department of Psychology.Google Scholar
  25. Dawson, G. (1988). Cerebral lateralization in autism: Clues to its role in language and affective development. In D. L. Molfese & S. J. Segalowitz (Eds.), Brain lateralization in children: Developmental implications (pp. 437–461). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  26. Dee, H. L. (1971). Auditory asymmetry and strength of manual preference. Cortex, 7, 236–245.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Delis, D, Simpson, G., & Knight, R. (1983). Reversed hemispheric organization in a left hander. Neuropsychologia, 21, 1, 13–24.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Deutsch, D. (1978). Pitch memory: An advantage for the left-handed. Science, 199, 559–560.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Dimond, S. (1972). The double brain. London: Churchill-Livingstone.Google Scholar
  30. Dronkers, N. E, & Knight, R. T. (1988). Right-sided neglect in a left-hander: Evidence for reversed hemispheric specialization of attention capacity. Neuropsychologia, 27(5), 729–735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Durost, W. N. (1934). The development of a battery of objective group tests of manual laterality with the results of their application to 1300 children. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 16, 223–336.Google Scholar
  32. Erne, R., Stone, S., & Izral, R. (1978). Spatial deficit in familial left-handed children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 47, 919–922.Google Scholar
  33. Feehan, M., Stanton, W. R., McGee, R., Silva, P. A., & Moffitt, T. E. (1990). Is there an association between lateral preference and delinquent behavior? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99, 198–201.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Filskov, S. B., & Catanese, R. A. (1986). Effects of sex and handedness on neuropsychological testing. In S. B. Filskov & T. J. Bokk (eds.), Handbook of clinical neuropsychology (pp. 198–212). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  35. Gabrielli, W. F., Jr., & Mednick, S. A. (1980). Sinistrality and delinquency. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 89(5), 654–661.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Geschwind, N. (1975). The apraxias: Neural mechanisms of disorders of learned movement. American Scientist, 63 188–195.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  37. Geschwind, N., & Behan, P. O. (1982). Left-handedness: Association with immune disease, migraine, and developmental learning disorder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 79, 5097–5100.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Geschwind, N., & Galaburda, A. M. (1985). Cerebral lateralization. Biological mechanisms, associations and pathology. I: A hypothesis and a program for research. Archives of Neurology, 42, 428–459.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Gloning, I., Gloning, K., Haub, G., & Quatember, R. (1969). Comparison of verbal behavior in right-handed and non right-handed patients with anatomically verified lesion of one hemisphere. Cortex, 5, 43–52.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Gloning, K., & Quatember, R. (1966). Statistical evidence of neuropsychological syndromes in left-handed and ambidextrous patients. Cortex, 2, 484–488.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Golden, C. J. (1978). Diagnosis and rehabilitation in clinical neuropsychology. Springfield, Ill.: Thomas.Google Scholar
  42. Golden, C. J., Hammeke, T. A. & Purisch, A. P. (1980). The Luria-Nebraska Neuropsychological Battery manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
  43. Gordon, N. (1986). Left-handedness and learning. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 28(5), 656–661.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Gregory, R., & Paul, J. (1980). The effects of handedness and writing posture on neuropsychological test results. Neuropsychologia, 18, 231–235.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Hammond, G. R., & Kaplan, R. J. (1982). Language lateralization and handedness: Estimates based on clinical data. Brain and Language, 16, 348–351.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Hardyck, C. (1977). Handedness and part-whole relationships: A replication. Cortex, 13, 177–183.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Hardyck, C., & Petrinovich, L. F. (1977). Left-handedness. Psychological Bulletin, 84(3), 385–403.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Hardyck, C., Petrinovich, L. F., & Goldman, R. D. (1976). Left-handedness and cognitive deficit. Cortex, 12, 266–279.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  49. Harris, L. J., & Carlson, D. F. (1988). Pathological left-handedness: An analysis of theories and evidence. In D. L. Molfese & S. J. Segalowitz (Eds.), Brain lateralization in children: Developmental implications (pp. 289–372). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  50. Harshman, R. A., Hampson, E., & Berenbaum, S. A. (1983). Individual differences in cognitive abilities and brain organization. Part 1: Sex and handedness differences in ability. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 37(1), 144–192.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Hecaen, H., & Sauguet, J. (1971). Cerebral dominance in left-handed subjects. Cortex, 7, 19–48.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Hecaen, H., DeAgostini, M., & Monzon-Montes, A. (1981). Cerebral organization in left-handers. Brain and Language, 12, 261–284.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Heilman, K. M., Coyle, J. M., Gonyea, E. F., & Geschwind, N. (1973). Apraxia and agraphia in a lefthander. Brain, 96, 21–28.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Henninger-Pechstedt, P. (1989a). Commissurotomy subjects show lateralized difference between manual and oral responding. Cortex, 25(2), 325–329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  55. Henninger, P. (1989b). Right-and left-handed musicians show differential hemispheric responsiveness to musical training, Caltech Biology Annual Report, #410.Google Scholar
  56. Henninger, P. (Submitted). Conditional handedness: Handedness changes in multiple personality disordered subject reflect shift in hemispheric dominance.Google Scholar
  57. Herrmann, D. J., & van Dyke, K. A. (1978). Handedness and the mental rotation of perceived patterns. Cortex, 14, 521–529.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Herron, J., Galin, D., Johnstone, J., & Ornstein, R. E. (1979). Cerebral specialization, writing posture, and motor control of writing in left-handers. Science, 205, 1285–1289.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  59. Ingram, D. (1975). Motor asymmetries in young children. Neuropsychologia, 13, 95–102.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Kimura, D. (1961a). Some effects of temporal-lobe damage on auditory perception. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 15(3), 156–165.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Kimura, D. (1961b.). Cerebral dominance and the perception of verbal stimuli. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 15(3), 166–171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Kimura, D. (1973). Manual activity during speaking. II. Left-handers. Neuropsychologia, 11, 51–55.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  63. Kimura, D. (1983). Sex differences in cerebral organization for speech and praxic functions. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 37, 19–35.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Kinsbourne, M. (1988). Sinistrality, brain organization, and cognitive deficits. In D. L. Molfese & S. J. Segalowitz (Eds.), Brain lateralization in children: Developmental implications (pp. 259–279). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  65. Knox, A. W., & Boone, D. R. (1970). Auditory laterality and tested handedness. Cortex, 6, 164–173.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Kolata, G. (1983). Math genius may have hormonal basis. Science, 222., 1312.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  67. Kopelman, M. D. (1982). Speech dominance, handedness and electro-convulsions. Psychological Medicine, 12, 667–670.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Kramer, M. A., Albrecht, S., & Miller, R. A. (1985). Handedness and the laterality of breast cancer in women. Nursing Research, 34(6), 333–337.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Lansdell, H. (1969). Verbal and nonverbal factors in right-hemisphere speech: Relation to early neurological history. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 69 734–738.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  70. LeMay, M., & Culebras, A. (1972). Human brain—morphologic differences in the hemispheres demonstrable by carotid arteriography New England Journal of Medicine., 287, 168–170.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  71. Levy, J. (1969). Possible basis for the evolution of lateral specialization of the human brain. Nature, 224, 614–615.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  72. Levy, J. (1976). A review of evidence for a genetic component in the determination of handedness. Behavior Genetics, 6(4), 429–453.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  73. Levy, J., & Nagylaki, T. (1972). A model for the genetics of handedness. Genetics, 72, 117–128.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Levy, J., & Reid, M. (1976). Variations in writing posture and cerebral organization. Science, 194, 337–339.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  75. Levy, J., & Trevarthen, C. (1976). Meta-control of hemispheric function in human split-brain patients. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2, 299–312.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  76. Levy, J., & Wagner, N. (1984). Handwriting posture, visuomotor integration, and lateralized reactiontime parameters. Human Neurobiology, 3, 157–161.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  77. Liederman, J., & Coryell, J. (1982). The origin of left hand preference: Pathological and nonpathological influences. Neuropsychologia, 20(6), 721–725.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  78. Lucas, J. A., Rosenstein, L. D., & Bigler, E. D. (1989). Handedness and language among the mentally retarded: Implications for the model of pathological left-handedness and gender differences in hemispheric specialization. Neuropsychologia, 27(5), 713–723.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  79. Luria, A. R. (1966). Higher cortical functions in man. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  80. Marino, M. E, & McKeever, W. F. (1982). The influence of sex and familial sinistrality on spatial laterality and spatial ability. Paper presented at the meeting of the International Neuropsychological Society, Pittsburgh.Google Scholar
  81. McCreadie, R. G., Crorie, J., Barron, E. T., & Winslow, G. S. (1982). The Nithsdale Schizophrenia Survey: III. Handedness and tardive dyskinesia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 591–594.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  82. McKeekan, E. R. L., & Lishman, W. A. (1975). Retest reliabilities and interrelationship of the Annett hand preference questionnaire and the Edinburgh handedness inventory. British Journal of Psychology, 66(1), 53–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  83. McKeever, W. F. (1979). Handwriting posture in left-handers: Sex, familial sinistrality and language laterality correlates. Neuropsychologia, 17, 6, 429–444.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  84. McKeever, W. F., & Hoff, A. L. (1979). Evidence of a possible isolation of left hemisphere visual and motor areas in sinistrals employing an inverted hand posture. Neuropsychologia, 17, 6, 445–455.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  85. McKeever, W. E, Seitz, K. S., Hoff, A. L., & Marino, M. F. (1983). Interacting sex and familial sinistrality characteristics influence both language lateralization and spatial ability in righthanders. Neuropsychologia, 22(6), 661–668.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  86. McManus, I. C. (1985). Handedness, language dominance and aphasia: A genetic model. Psychological Medicine, Monograph Supplement #8, Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  87. Mebert, C. J., & Michel, G. F. (1980). Handedness in artists. In J. Herron (Ed.), Neuropsychology of left-handedness (pp. 273–278). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  88. Miller, C. A. (1982). Degree of lateralization as a hierarchy of manual and cognitive skill levels. Neuropsychologia, 20(2), 155–162.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  89. Miller, E. (1971). Handedness and pattern of human ability. British Journal of Psychology, 62, 111–112.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  90. Milner, B. (1974). Hemispheric specialization: Scope and limits. In F. O. Schmitt & F. G. Worden (Eds.), The neurosciences: Third study program (pp. 75–89). Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  91. Milner, B., Branch, C., & Rasmussen, T. (1964). Observations on cerebral dominance. In A. V. S. deReuck & M. O’Connor (Eds.), Ciba Foundation symposium on disorders of language (pp. 200–214). London: Churchill.Google Scholar
  92. Myers, J. J. (1984). Right hemisphere language: Science or fiction? American Psychologist, 39(3), 315–320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  93. Naeser, M. A., & Borod, J. C. (1986). Aphasia in left-handers: Lesion site, lesion side, and hemispheric asymmetries on CT. Neurology, 36, 471–488.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  94. Natale, M., Gur, R. E., & Gur, R. C. (1983). Hemispheric asymmetries in processing emotional expressions. Neuropsychologia, 21(5), 555–565.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  95. Nebes, R. D. (1971). Handedness and the perception of part-whole relationships. Cortex, 7, 350–356.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  96. Newcombe, E, & Ratcliff, G. (1973). Handedness, speech lateralization, and ability. Neuropsychologia, 11, 4, 339–407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  97. Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh Inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  98. Penfield, W., & Roberts, L. (1959). Speech and brain mechanisms. Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
  99. Peters, M. (1985). Constraints in the performance of bimanual tasks and their expression in unskilled and skilled subjects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37A, 171–196.Google Scholar
  100. Peters, M. (1990). Phenotype in normal left-handers: An understanding of phenotype is the basis for understanding mechanism and inheritance of handedness. In S. Coren (Ed.), Left-handedness: Behavioral implications and anomalies (pp. 167–192). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  101. Peters, M., & Durding, B. (1978). Handedness measured by fingertapping: A continuous variable. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 32, 257–261.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  102. Peters, M., & Durding, B. (1979). Left-handers and right-handers compared on a motor task. Journal of Motor Behavior, 11, 103–111.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  103. Peterson, J. M. (1979). Left-handedness: Differences between student artists and scientists. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 48, 961–962.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  104. Peterson, J. M., & Lansky, L. M. (1974). Left-handedness among architects: Some facts and speculation. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 38, 547–550.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  105. Peterson, J. M., & Lansky, L. M. (1980). Success in architecture: Handedness and visual thinking. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 50, 1139–1143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  106. Raczkowski, D, Kalat, J. W., & Nebes, R. (1974). Reliability and validity of some handedness questionnaire items. Neuropsychologia, 12, 1, 43–47.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  107. Rasmussen, T., & Milner, B. (1975). Clinical and surgical studies of the cerebral speech areas in man. In K. J. Zulch, O. Creutzfeldt, & G. Galbraith (Eds.), Otfrid Foerster symposium on cerebral localization (pp. 238–257). Berlin: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  108. Reitan, R. M. (1955). Investigation of the validity of Halstead’s measures of biological intelligence. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 73, 28–35.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  109. Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1985). The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Battery: Theory and clinical interpretation, Tucson: Neuropsychology Press.Google Scholar
  110. Reitan, R. M., Horn, J., & Wolfson, D. (1988). Verbal processing by the brain. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 10(1), 400–408.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  111. Rosenstein, L. D., Van Sickle, L. F. (1991, February). Artificial depression of left-hand finger-tapping rates: A critical evaluation of the Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Finger Tapping test instrument. Poster presented at the meeting of the International Neuropsychological Society, Austin.Google Scholar
  112. Satz, P. (1972). Pathological left-handedness: An explanatory model. Cortex, 8, 121–135.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  113. Satz, P. (1979). A text of some models of hemispheric speech organization in the left-and right-handed. Science, 203, 1131–1133.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  114. Satz, P. (1980). Incidence of aphasia in left-handers: A test of some hypothetical models of cerebral speech organization. In J. Herron (Ed.), Neuropsychology of left-handedness (pp. 189–197). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  115. Satz, P, Achenbach, K., & Fennell, E. (1967). Correlations between assessed manual laterality and predicted speech laterality in a normal population. Neuropsychologia, 5, 4, 295–310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  116. Satz, P., Orsini, D. L., Saslow, E., & Henry, R. (1985). The pathological left-handedness syndrome. Brain and Cognition, 4, 27–46.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  117. Satz, P, Soper, H. V, & Orsini, D. (1988). Human hand preference: Three nondextral subtypes. In D. L. Molfese & S. J. Segalowitz (Eds.), Brain lateralization in children: Developmental implications (pp. 281–287). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
  118. Searleman, A., Tweedy, J., & Springer, S. P (1979). Interrelationships among subject variables believed to predict cerebral organization. Brain and Language, 7, 267–276.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  119. Searleman, A., Porac, C., & Coren, S. (1984). Writing hand posture and four indexes of lateral preference. Brain and Cognition, 3, 86–93.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  120. Searleman, A., Porac, C., & Coren, S. (1989). Relationship between birth order, birth stress, and lateral preferences: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 105(3), 397–408.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  121. Segalowitz, S. J., & Bryden, M. P. (1983). Individual differences in hemispheric representation of language. In S. J. Segalowitz (Ed.), Language functions and brain organization (pp. 341–372). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  122. Seidenberg, M., Gamache, M. P., Beck, N. C., Smith, M., Giordani, B., Berent, S., Sackellares, J. C., & Boll, T. J. (1984). Subject variables and performance on the Halstead neuropsychological test battery: A multivariate analysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 52(4), 658–662.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  123. Seltzer, B., Burres, M. S. K., & Sherwin, I. (1984). Left-handedness in early and late onset dementia. Neurology, 34, 367–369.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  124. Serafetinides, E. A., Hoare, R. D., & Driver, M. V (1965). Intracarotid sodium amylobarbitone and cerebral dominance for speech and consciousness. Brain, 88, 107–130.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  125. Smith, L. C., & Moscovitch, M. (1979). Writing posture, hemispheric control of movement and cerebral dominance in individuals with inverted and noninverted hand postures during writing. Neuropsychologia, 17, 6, 637–644.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  126. Sperry, R. W (1974). Lateral specialization in the surgically separated hemispheres. In F. O. Schmitt & F. G. Worden (Eds.), The neurosciences: Third study program (Vol. 3, pp. 5–19). Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  127. Springer, S. P., & Deutsch, G. (1989). Left brain, right brain (3rd ed.). San Francisco: Freeman.Google Scholar
  128. Steenhuis, R. E., & Bryden, M. P. (1988, June). The relation between hand preference and hand performance. In M. Schwartz (Chair), Handedness: Its measurement, performance and distribution in normal and problem populations. Symposium conducted at the meeting of the Canadian Psychological Association, Montreal.Google Scholar
  129. Strauss, E. (1986). Hand, foot, eye and ear preferences and performance on a dichotic listening test. Cortex, 22, 475–482.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  130. Strauss, E., & Wada, J. (1983). Lateral preferences and cerebral speech dominance. Cortex, 19, 165–177.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  131. Strauss, E., & Wada, J. (1988). Hand preference and proficiency and cerebral speech dominance determined by the carotid amytal test. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 10(2), 169–174.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  132. Strauss, E., Wada, J., & Kosaka, B. (1984). Writing hand posture and cerebral dominance for speech. Cortex, 20, 143–147.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  133. Sussman, H. M. (1971). The laterality effect in lingual-auditory tracking. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 496(2), 1874–1880.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  134. Tapley, S. M., & Bryden, M. P. (1983). Handwriting position and hemispheric asymmetry in righthanders. Neuropsychologia, 21(2), 129–138.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  135. Tapley, S. M., & Bryden, M. P. (1985). A group test for the assessment of performance between the hands. Neuropsychologia, 23(2), 215–221.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  136. Teng, E. L., Lee, P., Yang, K., & Chang, P. C. (1976). Handedness in a Chinese population: Biological, social and pathological factors. Science, 193, 1148–1150.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  137. Thompson, L. L., Heaton, R. K., Matthews, C. G., & Grant, I. (1987). Comparison of preferred and nonpreferred hand performance on four neuropsychological motor tasks. The Clinical Neuropsy-chologist, 2(4), 324–334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  138. Weber, A. M., & Bradshaw, J. L. (1981). Levy and Reid’s neurological model in relation to writing hand/posture: An evaluation. Psychological Bulletin, 90, 74–88.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  139. Witelson, S. F. (1987). Neurobiological aspects of language in children. Child Development, 58, 653–688.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  140. Witelson, S. F. (1989). Hand and sex differences in the isthmus and genu of the human corpus callosum: A postmortem morphological study. Brain, 112, 799–835.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  141. Zaidel, E. (1976). Lexical organization in the right hemisphere. In P. A. Buser & A. Rougeul-Buser (Eds.), Cerebral correlates of conscious experience (pp. 177–197). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  142. Zangwill, O. (1960). Cerebral dominance and its relation to psychological function. Edinburgh: Oliver & Boyd.Google Scholar
  143. Zurif, E. B., & Bryden, M. P. (1969). Familial handedness in left-right differences in auditory and visual perception. Neuropsychologia, 7, 179–187.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1992

Authors and Affiliations

  • Polly Henninger
    • 1
  1. 1.Division of Biology 156-29California Institute of TechnologyPasadenaUSA

Personalised recommendations