Mechanisms of Individual Discrimination in Hamsters

  • Robert E. Johnston

Abstract

The capacity to discriminate between individuals has been demonstrated in a variety of animal species (see Halpin, 1980, for a recent review). Among mammals, the cues that have been studied most frequently as sources of individually distinct information have been olfactory; for example, mice, rats, gerbils, dogs, wolves and mongooses all have been shown to be capable of discriminating between the odors of different individuals of the same species (Bowers and Alexander, 1967; Carr et al., 1970; Halpin, 1976; Brown and Johnston, this volume; Rasa, 1973). In most of these studies it is the capacity to discriminate between individuals that has been demonstrated, either in a discrimination learning task or in an habituation-dishabituation paradigm. Neither of these tasks demonstrate that animals actually use individual discrimination in their normal social interactions. The habituation paradigm is preferable in this regard, since the animals tested in this type of task do spontaneously notice and pay attention to differences in the odors of individuals (e.g., see Brown and Johnston, this volume). One situation in which functional use of individual discrimination has been repeatedly demonstrated among mammals is the recognition of young in a variety of ungulates, in which both odors and vocalizations have been shown to be important (Klopfer and Gamble, 1967; Espmark, 1971).

Keywords

Olfactory System Individual Recognition Olfactory Mucosa Harderian Gland Main Olfactory Bulb 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Bellringer, J. F., Pratt, H. P. M., and Keverne, E. B., 1980, Involvement of the vomeronasal organ and prolactin in pheromonal induction of delayed implantation in mice, J. Reprod. Fert., 59:223.Google Scholar
  2. Bowers, J. M. and Alexander, B. K., 1967, Mice: individual recognition by olfactory cues, Science 158: 1208.Google Scholar
  3. Bunnell, B., Boland, B. D., and Dewsbury, D. A., 1977, Copulatory behavior of golden hamsters, Mesocricetus auratus, Behaviour,61:180.Google Scholar
  4. Carr, W. J., Krames, L. and Costanzo, D. J., 1970, Previous sexual experience and olfactory preference for novel versus original sex partners in rats, J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol., 71:216.Google Scholar
  5. Clabough, J. W. and Norvell, J. E., 1973, Effects of castration, blinding, and the pineal gland on the Harderian glands of the male golden hamster, Neuroendocrinology 12: 344.Google Scholar
  6. Dewsbury, D. A., 1981, Effects of novelty on copulatory behavior: The Coolidge effect and related phenomena, Psych. Bull., 89: 464Google Scholar
  7. Eichenbaum, H., Shedlack, K. J. and Eckmann, K. W., 1980, Thalamocortical mechanisms in odor-guided behavior. I. Effects of lesions of the mediodorsal thalamic nucleus and frontal cortex on olfactory discrimination in the rat, Brain Behay. Evol.,17:255.Google Scholar
  8. Espmark, Y., 1971, Individual recognition by voice in reindeer mother-young relationship. Field observations and playback experiments, Behaviour, 40: 295.Google Scholar
  9. Fleming, A., Vaccarino, F., Tambosso, L., and Chee, P., 1979, Vomeronasal and olfactory modulation of maternal behavior in the rat, Science, 203: 372.Google Scholar
  10. Halpin, Z. T., 1976, The role of individual recognition by odors in social interactions of the mongolian gerbil (Meriones unguiculatus), Behaviour, 48:117.Google Scholar
  11. Halpin, Z. T., 1980, Individual odors and individual recognition: review and commentary, Biol. Behay., 5:233.Google Scholar
  12. Hamilton, J. B., and Montagna, W., 1950, The sebaceous glands of the hamster. I. Morphological effects of androgens on integumentary structures, Amer. J. Anat., 86:191.Google Scholar
  13. Johnston, R. E., 1977, Scent marking by male hamsters. I. Effects of odors and social encounters, Z. Tierpsychol., 37:75.Google Scholar
  14. Johnston, R. E., 1977a, The causation of two scent marking behaviors in female golden hamsters, Anim. Behay., 25:317.Google Scholar
  15. Johnston, R. E., 1977b, Sex pheromones in golden hamsters, in: “Chemical Signals in Vertebrates,” D. Müller-Schwarze and M. M. Mozell, eds., Plenum, NY.Google Scholar
  16. Kaneko, N., Debski, E. A., Wilson, M. C., Whitten, W. K., 1980, Puberty acceleration in mice. II. Evidence that the vomeronasal organ is a receptor for the primer pheromone in male mouse urine, Biol. Reprod., 22:873.Google Scholar
  17. Klopfer, P. H., and Gamble, J., 1967, Maternal “imprinting” in goats: The role of chemical senses, Z. Tierpsychol., 23:588.Google Scholar
  18. Landauer, M. R., Liu, S., and Goldberg, N., 1980, Responses of male hamsters to the ear gland secretions of conspecifics, Physiol. Behay., 24:1023.Google Scholar
  19. Meredith, M., Marques, D. M., O’Connell, R. J., and Stern, F. L., 1980, Vomeronasal pump: significance for male hamster sexual behavior, Science 207: 1224.Google Scholar
  20. Murphy, M. R., 1980, Sexual preferences of male hamsters: importance of preweaning and adult experience, vaginal secretion and olfactory or vomeronasal sensation, Behay. Neural Biol., 30: 2–32.Google Scholar
  21. Payne, A. P., McGadey, J., Moore, M. R., and Thompson, G., 1977, Androgenic control of the Harderian gland in the male golden hamster, J. Endocr., 75: 73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Payne, A. P., Rasa, O. A. E., 1973, Marking behaviour and its social significance in the African Dwarf Mongoose, Helogale undulata rufula, Z. Tierpsychol., 32: 293.Google Scholar
  23. Sapolsky, R. M. and Eichenbaum, H., 1980, Thalamocortical mechanisms in odor-guided behavior. II. Effects of lesions of the medio-dorsal thalamic nucleus and frontal cortex on odor preferences and sexual behavior in the hamster, Brain Behay. Evol., 17:276.Google Scholar
  24. Thiessen, D. D., 1977, Thermoenergetics and the evolution of pheromone communication, Progress in Psychobiology and Physiological Psychology, 7:91Google Scholar
  25. Thomas, J. A. and Birney, E. C., 1979, Parental care and mating system of the prairie vole, Microtus ochrogaster, Behay. Ecol. Sociobiol., 5:171.Google Scholar
  26. Williams, R. J., 1956, “Biochemical Individuality.”, Wiley, NY.Google Scholar
  27. Wilson, J. R., Kuehn, R. E. and Beach, F. A., 1963, Modification in the sexual behavior of male rats produced by changing the stimulus female, J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol., 56:636.Google Scholar
  28. Winans, S. S. and Powers, J. B., 1977, Olfactory and vomeronasal deafferentation of male hamsters: histological and behavioral analyses, Brain Research, 126: 325.Google Scholar
  29. Wysocki, C. J., 1979, Neurobehavioral evidence for the involvement of the vomeronasal system in mammalian reproduction, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 3: 301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Wysocki, C. J., 1982, The vomeronasal organ: its influence upon reproductive behavior and underlying endocrine systems, In: “Olfaction and Endocrine Processes,” W. Breipohl, Ed., Information Retrieval Limited, London.Google Scholar
  31. Wysocki, C. J., Wellington, J. L. and Beauchamp, G. K., 1980, Access of urinary nonvolatiles to the mammalian vomeronasal organ, Science, 207: 781.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1983

Authors and Affiliations

  • Robert E. Johnston
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of PsychologyCornell UniversityIthacaUSA

Personalised recommendations