From Spatiotemporal Tuning to Velocity Analysis
Abstract
One of the authors (MW) had the privilege of working with Hisako Ikeda between 1970 and 1975, and carried out extensive single cell recording studies in the retina, LGN and visual cortex of cats. Ikeda and Wright (1976) also conducted some of the earliest studies on animal models of amblyopia, in which they looked at the effects of surgically induced squint in kittens on the responsiveness of single cells in the LGN. This work was interpreted within a framework of parallel processing streams in the visual pathways. All these issues are very much still current concerns of the Vision Research community. One of these topics, parallel processing and spatiotemporal tuning of cortical neurons is the subject of current research at Brunel University. In this work, we try to develop models and explanations of human vision, for instance, human motion perception, which are firmly rooted in physiology, and make use of fundamental neurophysiological findings in deriving explanations of human visual performance.
Keywords
Spatial Frequency Visual Cortex Receptive Field Temporal Frequency Motion PerceptionPreview
Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.
References
- Adelson, E.H. and Movshon, J.A. (1982). Phenomenal coherence of moving visual patterns, Nature 300:523–525.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Blasdel, G.G. and Salama, G. (1986) Voltage-sensitive dyes reveal a modular organisation in monkey striate cortex, Nature 321:579–585.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bonhoffer, T. and Grinvald, A. (1991) Iso-orientation domains in cat visual cortex are arranged in pinwheel-like patterns, Nature 353:429–431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Enroth-Cugell, C. and Robson, J.G. (1966) The contrast sensitivity of retinal ganglion cells in the cat, J. Physiol. 187:517–552.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Gurney, K.N. and Wright, M.J. (1992). A self-organising neural network model of image velocity encoding, Biological Cybernetics 68:173–181.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hubel, D. H. and Wiesel, T.N. (1962) Receptive fields, functional architecture and binocular interaction in the cat’s visual cortex, J. Physiol. 160:106–154.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Hubel, D.H. and Wiesel, T.N. (1977). The Ferrier Lecture: Functional architecture of macaque monkey visual cortex, Proc. Roy Soc. Lond. B. 198:1–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Ikeda, H. and Wright, M.J. (1972) Receptive field organisation of “sustained” and “transient” retinal ganglion cells which subserve different functional roles, J. Physiol 227:769–800.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Ikeda, H. and Wright, M.J. (1975) Spatial and temporal properties of “sustained” and “transient” neurons in area 17 of the cat’s visual cortex, Exp. Brain Res. 22:362–382Google Scholar
- Ikeda, H. and Wright, M.J. (1975) Retinotopic distribution, visual latency and orientation tuning of “sustained” and “transient” neurons in area 17 of the cat, Exp. Brain Res. 22:385–398.Google Scholar
- Ikeda, H. and Wright, M.J. (1976) Properties of LGN cells in kittens reared with convergent squint: a neurophysiological demonstration of amblyopia. Exp. Brain Res. 25:63–77.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Johnston, A and Wright, M.J. (1986) Matching velocity in central and peripheral vision, Vision Res. 26:1099–1109PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kohonen, T. (1982) Self-organised formation of topologically correct feature maps., Biological Cybernetics 43:59–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kohonen, T. (1984) Self-organisation and associative memory. Springer, Berlin, Heidelberg, New York.Google Scholar
- Merigan, W.H. and Eskin, T.A. (1986) Spatio-temporal vision of macaques with severe loss of Pb retinal ganglion cells, Vision Res. 26:1751–1761.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Merigan, W.H. and Maunsell, J.H.R. (1993). How parallel are the primate visual pathways?, Ann Rev. Neurosci., 16, 369–402.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Merigan, W.H. Byrne, C.E. and Maunsell, J.H.R. (1991) Does primate motion perception depend on the magnocellular pathway?, Neurosci. 11:3422–3429.Google Scholar
- Movshon, J.A., Adelson, E.H., Gizzi, M.S. and Newsome, W.T. (1985). The analysis of moving visual patterns. In Chagas, C. and Gross, C.G. (Eds.) Pattern recognition mechanisms (pp 117-151) Vatican Press.Google Scholar
- Mustari, M.J., Bullier, J. and Henry, G.H. (1981). Comparison of the response properties of three types of monosynaptic S cell in cat striate cortex, J. Neurophysiol. 47:15–741.Google Scholar
- Newsome, W.T. and Pare E.B. (1988) A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal area (MT), J. Neurosci. 8:2201–2211.PubMedGoogle Scholar
- Henry, G.H. (1986) Streaming in the striate cortex. In Visual Neuroscience, eds. Pettigrew, J.D., Sanderson, K.J. and Levick, W.R. pp 260–279. Cambridge, CUP.Google Scholar
- Smith, A.T. and Edgar, G.K. (1991). The separability of temporal frequency and velocity, Vision Res. 31, 321–326.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Thompson. P (1982). The perceived rate of movement depends on contrast. Vision Res. 22:377–380.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Wright, M.J. and Gurney, K.N. (1992) Lower threshold of motion for one and two dimensional patterns in central and peripheral vision, Vision Res. 32:121–134.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Zihl, J. Cramon, D. von and Mai, N. (1983) Selective disturbance of movement vision after bilateral brain damage, Brain 106:313–340.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Zeki, S. (1993) A vision of the brain. Blackwell Scientific Publications, Oxford.Google Scholar