Playback as a Tool for Studying Contests between Social Groups

  • Karen McComb
Chapter
Part of the NATO ASI Series book series (NSSA, volume 228)

Abstract

When animals compete over resources, opponents are rarely equally matched (Parker 1974; Maynard Smith and Parker 1976; Parker and Rubenstein 1981; Maynard Smith 1982). Apart from differences in ownership of the resource at the outset of the contest (Davies 1978; Packer and Pusey 1982) and in the value of the resource to each opponent (Riechert 1979; Austad 1983), opponents differ in their ability to acquire / defend the resource — Resource Holding Potential (Parker 1974; Riechert 1978; Sigurjonsdottir and Parker 1981). Games theory models have shown that where fights endanger future survival and reproductive success, individuals should monitor the value of the resource and the Resource Holding Potential of their opponent, and withdraw without escalation if they would be likely to lose an ensuing fight (Parker 1974; Parker and Rubenstein 1981).

Keywords

Opposing Group Wolf Pack Resource Holding African Lion Social Mammal 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Arak, A. 1983. Sexual selection by male-male competition in natterjack toad choruses. Nature, 306, 261–262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Brown, J.L. 1987. Helping and Communal Breeding in Birds. Princeton University Press, Princeton, New Jersey.Google Scholar
  3. Brown, E.D. and Veltman, C.J. 1987. Ethogram of the Australian magpie (Gymnorhina tibicen) in comparison to other Cracticidae and Corvus species. Ethology, 76, 309–333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Brown, E.D., Farabaugh, S.M. and Veltman, C.J. 1988. Song sharing in a group-living songbird, the Australian magpie, Gymnorhina tibicen. Part I: Vocal sharing within and among social groups. Behaviour, 104, 1–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Cheney, D.L. 1986. Interactions and relationships between groups. In: Primate Societies. (Ed. by B.B. Smuts, D.L. Cheney, R.M. Seyfarth, R.W. Wrangham and T.T. Strhsaker ), pp. 267–281. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
  6. Clutton-Brock, T.H. and Albon, S.D. 1979. The roaring of red deer and the evolution of honest advertisement. Behaviour, 69, 145–169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Clutton-Brock, T.H., Albon, S.D. and Guinness, F.E. 1988. Reproductive success in male and female red deer. In: Reproductive Success: Studies of Individual Variation in Contrasting Breeding Systems. (Ed. by T.H. Clutton-Brock ), pp. 325–343. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
  8. Davies, N.B. 1978. Territorial defence in the speckled wood butterfly Pararge aegeria: the resident always wins. Anim. Behay., 26, 138–147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Davies, N.B. and Halliday, T.R. 1978. Deep croaks and fighting assessment in toads (Bufo bufo). Nature, 274, 683–685.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. East, M.L. and Hofer, H. 1991. Loud calling in a female dominated mammalian society II: Behavioural contexts and functions of whooping of spotted hyaenas, Crocuta crocuta. Anim. Behay., 42, 651–669.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Green, A.J. 1990. Determinants of chorus participation and the effects of size, weight and competition on advertisement calling in the tungara frog, Physalaemus pustulosus (Leptodactylidae). Anim. Behay., 39, 620–638.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Harrington, F.H. and Mech, L.D. 1979. Wolf howling and its role in territory maintenance. Behaviour, 68, 207–249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Harrington, F.H. and Mech, L.D. 1983. Wolf pack spacing: howling as a territory-independent spacing mechanism in a territorial population. Behay. Ecol. Sociobiol., 12, 161–168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Harrington, F.H. 1989. Chorus howling by wolves: acoustic structure, pack size and the beau geste effect. Bioacoustics, 2, 117–136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Krebs, J.R., Ashcroft, R. and Webber, M. 1978. Song repertoires and territory defense in the great tit. Nature, 271, 539–542.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Ligon, J.D. and Ligon, S.H. 1982. The cooperative breeding behavior of the Green woodhoopoe. Scient. Am., 247 (1), 106–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Maynard Smith, J. and Parker, G.A. 1976. The logic of asymmetric contests. Anim. Behay., 24, 159–175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Maynard Smith, J. 1982. Evolution and the Theory of Games. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Miller, G.A. 1956. The magical number seven plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psych. Rev., 63, 81–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Mitani, J.C. 1985. Gibbon duets and intergroup spacing. Behaviour, 92, 59–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. McComb, K., Packer, C. and Pusey, A. in prep. Roaring and assessment in African lions (Panthera leo).Google Scholar
  22. Parker, G.A. 1974. Assessment strategy and the evolution of fighting behaviour. J. Theor. Biol., 74, 223–243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Parker, G.A. and Rubenstein, D.I. 1981. Role assessment, reserve strategy and acquisition of information in asymmetric animal conflicts. Anim. Behay., 29, 221–240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Packer, C. and Pusey, A. 1985. Asymmetric contests in social mammals: respect, manipulation and age-specific aspects. In: Evolution: Essays in Honour of John Maynard Smith. (Ed. by J.J. Greenwood & M.Slatkin ), pp. 173–186. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.Google Scholar
  25. Packer, C., Scheel, D. and Pusey, A.E. 1990. Why lions form groups: food is not enough. Am. Nat., 136, 1–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Raemaekers, J.J. and Raemaekers, P.M. 1984. The Ooaa duet of the gibbon (Hylobates lar): a group call which triggers other groups to respond in kind. Folia primatol., 42, 209–215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Riechert, S.E. 1978. Games spiders play: behavioral variability in territorial disputes. Behay. Ecol. Sociobiol., 3, 135–162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Riechert, S.E. 1979. Games spiders play II: resource assessment strategies. Behay. Ecol. Sociobiol., 6, 121–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Robertson, J.G.M. 1986. Male territoriality, fighting and assessment of fighting ability in the Australian frog (Uperoleia rugosa). Anim. Behay., 34, 763–772.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Robinson, S.K. 1985. Fighting and assessment in the yellow-rumped cacique (Cacicus cela). Behay. Ecol. Sociobiol., 18, 39–44.Google Scholar
  31. Ryan, M.J., Tuttle, M.D. and Taft, L.K. 1981. The costs and benefits of frog chorusing behaviour. Behay. Ecol. Sociobiol., 8, 273–278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Schaller, G.B. 1972. The Serengeti Lion: A Study of Predator-Prey Relations. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.Google Scholar
  33. Sekulic, R. 1982. The function of howling in red howler monkeys (Alouatta seniculus). Behaviour, 81, 38–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Sigurjonsdottir, H. and Parker, G.A. 1981. Dung fly struggles: evidence for assessment strategy. Behay. Ecol. Sociobiol., 8, 219–230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Wagner, W.E. 1989. Graded aggressive signals in Blanchard’s cricket frog: vocal responses to opponent proximity and size. Anim. Behay., 38, 1025–1038.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Waser, P. 1975. Experimental playbacks show mediation of intergroup avoidance in a forest monkey. Nature, 225, 56–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Whitehead, J.M. 1987. Vocally mediated reciprocity between neighbouring groups of mantled howler monkeys Alouatta palliata palliata. Anim. Behay., 35, 1615–1627.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1992

Authors and Affiliations

  • Karen McComb
    • 1
  1. 1.Large Animal Research Group, Department of ZoologyUniversity of CambridgeCambridgeUK

Personalised recommendations