The Creative Construction of Rationality

A Paradox?
  • David Moshman
  • Leslie E. Lukin
Chapter
Part of the Perspectives on Individual Differences book series (PIDF)

Abstract

Is the development of rationality a creative process? Is there something paradoxical about the idea that creativity can result in increasing rationality? We will argue in this chapter that rationality is indeed creatively constructed by the developing individual. If one thinks of creativity as consisting of unconstrained flights of the imagination and of rationality as strict adherence to mechanical rules of reasoning, there is indeed something paradoxical about this conclusion. The paradox dissolves, however, upon reconsideration of the definitions of both rationality and creativity.

Keywords

Cognitive Model Creative Process Inductive Reasoning Deductive Reasoning Logical Necessity 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  2. Bereiter, C. (1985). Toward a solution of the learning paradox. Review of Educational Research, 55, 201–226.Google Scholar
  3. Brain, M. D. S. (1978). On the relation between the natural logic of reasoning and standard logic. Psychological Review, 85, 1–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Braine, M. D. S., and Rumain, B. (1983). Logical reasoning. In J. H. Flavell and E. M. Markman (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Cognitive development (pp. 263–340 ). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  5. Broughton, J. (1978). Development of concepts of self, mind, reality, and knowledge. In W. Damon (Ed.), Social cognition (pp. 75–100 ). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
  6. Campbell, R. L., and Bickhard, M. H. (1986). Knowing levels and developmental stages. Basel: Karger.Google Scholar
  7. Campbell, R. L., and Bickhard, M. H. (1987). A deconstruction of Fodor’s anticonstructivism. Human Development, 30, 4859.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Clinchy, B. M., and Mansfield, A. F. ( 1986, April). The child’s discovery of the role of the knower in the known. Paper pre-Google Scholar
  9. sented at the meeting of the Jean Piaget Society, Philadelphia. Cummins, J. (1978). Language and children’s ability to evaluate contradictions and tautologies: A critique of Osherson and Markman’s findings. Child Development, 49, 895–897CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Fabricius, W. V., Sophian, C., and Wellman, H. M. (1987). Young children’s sensitivity to logical necessity in their inferential search behavior. Child Development, 58, 409–423PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Flavell, J. H. (1986). The development of children’s knowledge about the appearance-reality distinction. American Psychologist, 41, 418–425.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L., and Flavell, E. R. (1986). Development of knowledge about the appearance-reality distinction. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 51(1, Serial No. 212 ).Google Scholar
  13. Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L., Wahl, K. E., and Flavell, E. R. (1987). The effects of question clarification and memory aids on young children’s performance on appearance-reality tasks. Cognitive Development, 2, 127–144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Hawkins, J., Pea, R. D., Glick, J., and Scribner, S. (1984). “Merds that laugh don’t like mushrooms”: Evidence for deductive reasoning by preschoolers. Developmental Psychology, 20, 584–594.Google Scholar
  15. James, W. (1890/1950). The principles of psychology (Vol. 2). New York: Dover.Google Scholar
  16. Keil, F. C. (1984). Mechanisms of cognitive development and the structure of knowledge. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Mechanisms of cognitive development (pp. 81–99 ). New York: Freeman.Google Scholar
  17. King, P. M., Kitchener, K. S., Davison, M. L., Parker, C. A., and Wood, P. K. (1983). The justification of beliefs in young adults: A longitudinal study. Human Development, 26, 106–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Kitchener, K. S., and King, P. M. (1981). Reflective judgment: Concepts of justification and their relationship to age and education. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 2, 89–116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Kitchener, K. S., and Wood, P. K. (1987). Development of concepts of justification in German university students. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 10, 171–185.Google Scholar
  20. Kitchener, R. F. (1978). Epigenesis: The role of biological models in developmental psychology. Human Development, 21, 141–160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Kitchener, R. F. (1980). Predetermined versus probabilistic epigenesis: A reply to Lerner. Human Development, 23, 73–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Klayman, J., and Ha, Y. (1987). Confirmation, disconfirmation, and information in hypothesis testing. Psychological Review, 94, 211–228.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Komatsu, L. K., and Galotti, K. M. (1986). Children’s reasoning about social, physical, and logical regularities: A look at two worlds. Child Development, 57, 413–420.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Kuhn, D., and Phelps, E. (1979). A methodology for observing development of a formal reasoning strategy. In D. Kuhn (Ed.), Intellectual development beyond childhood (pp. 4557 ). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
  25. Lerner, R. M. (1986). Concepts and theories of human development ( 2nd ed. ). New York: Random House.Google Scholar
  26. Moshman, D. (1979a). Development of formal hypothesis-testing ability. Developmental Psychology, 15, 104–112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Moshman, D. (1979b). To really get ahead, get a metatheory. In D. Kuhn (Ed.), Intellectual development beyond childhood (pp. 59–68 ). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
  28. Moshman, D. (1982). Exogenous, endogenous, and dialectical constructivism. Developmental Review, 2, 371–384CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Moshman, D. (1989). The development of metalogical understanding. In W. F. Overton (Ed.), Reasoning, necessity, and logic: Developmental perspectives. Hillsdale, NJ: ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
  30. Moshman, D., and Franks, B. A. (1986). Development of the concept of inferential validity. Child Development, 57, 153–165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Moshman, D., and Hoover, L. M. (1989). Rationality as a goal of psychotherapy. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 3, 3151.Google Scholar
  32. Moshman, D., and Timmons, M. (1982). The construction of logical necessity. Human Development, 25, 309–323CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. O’Brien, D. P. (1987). The development of conditional reason-ing: An iffy proposition. In H. W. Reese (Ed.), Advances in Child Development and Behavior (Vol. 20, pp. 61–90 ). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. O’Brien, D. P., Costa, G., and Overton, W. F. (1986). Evaluations of causal and conditional hypotheses. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38A, 493–512.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  35. Osherson, D. N., and Markman, E. (1975). Language and the ability to evaluate contradictions and tautologies. Cognition, 3, 213–226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Overton, W. F., Ward, S. L., Noveck, I. A., Black, J., and O’Brien, D. P. (1987). Form and content in the development of deductive reasoning. Developmental Psychology, 23, 2230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Piaget, J. (1985). The equilibration of cognitive structures: The central problem of intellectual development. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
  38. Piaget, J. (1986). Essay on necessity. Human Development, 29, 301–314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Pieraut-Le Bonniec, G. (1980). The development of modal reasoning: Genesis of necessity and possibility notions. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  40. Politzer, G. (1986). Laws of language use and formal logic. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 15, 47–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Popper, K. (1959). The logic of scientific discovery. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
  42. Skov, R. B., and Sherman, S. J. (1986). Information-gathering processes: Diagnosticity, hypothesis-confirmatory strategies, and perceived hypothesis confirmation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 22, 93–121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Sodian, B., and Wimmer, H. (1987). Children’s understanding of inference as a source of knowledge. Child Development, 58, 424–433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Somerville, S. C., Hadkinson, B. A., and Greenberg, C. (1979). Two levels of inferential behavior in young children. Child Development, 50, 119–131.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. von Glasersfeld, E. (1979). Radical constructivism and Piaget’s concept of knowledge. In F. B. Murray (Ed.), The impact of Piagetian theory: On education, philosophy, psychiatry, and psychology (pp. 109–122 )Google Scholar
  46. Baltimore: University Park Press. Walker, L. J. (1982). The sequentiality of Kohlberg’s stages of moral development. Child Development, 53, 1330–1336Google Scholar
  47. Wason, P. C. (1983). Realism and rationality in the selection task. In J. St. B. T. Evans (Ed.), Thinking and reasoning: Psychological approaches. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
  48. Welfel, E. R., and Davison, M. L. (1986). The development of reflective judgment during the college years: A 4-year longitudinal study. Journal of College Student Personnel, 27, 209–216.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1989

Authors and Affiliations

  • David Moshman
    • 1
  • Leslie E. Lukin
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of Educational PsychologyUniversity of NebraskaLincolnUSA

Personalised recommendations