The Psychophysical Study of Visual Motion Aftereffect Rate in Monkeys

  • Thomas R. Scott
  • W. Lloyd Milligan

Abstract

Visual aftereffect of motion is the apparent motion of an objectively stationary stimulus pattern induced by prior viewing of a moving pattern. The aftereffect is probably always in a direction opposite to that of the eliciting motion. Aristotle was the first to describe the phenomenon (see Hutchins, 1952). His example was the apparent motion of stationary objects observed after steady fixation of a swiftly flowing stream. Purkinje (1825) described the aftereffect, which he observed after viewing a cavalry procession, and suggested that habitual eye movements learned during the eliciting period continued during the viewing of the stationary object. Helmholtz (1867) and Wundt (1874) were among those who supported this explanation of aftereffect. Plateau (1850) introduced the rotating spiral as an eliciting stimulus. The rotating spiral appears to move in toward the center or out away from it, depending upon the direction of rotation. The aftereffect is one of expansion in the former case and contraction in the latter. Plateau’s observation defeated Purkinje’s theory since the eyes cannot move in all directions at once. There is a voluminous 19th century literature, which includes attempts to illustrate analogous effects in other senses, e. g., Dvorak (1871). Mach (1875) was the first to propose a retinal locus for the physiologic processes involved in the aftereffect. Several ingenious retinal theories were subsequently devised to account for the aftereffect (e. g., Exner, 1876). A good review of the older literature is to be found in Wohlgemuth (1911) who suggested that the aftereffect might be a useful index to central nervous system function. A more recent summary is given in an excellent book by Holland (1965).

Keywords

Test Stimulus Lever Press Psychophysical Study Motion Aftereffect Interocular Transfer 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Anderson, T. W., P. J. McCarthy, and J. W. Tukey. 1946. “Staircase” Methods of Sensitivity Testing. NAVORD Report 65–46.Google Scholar
  2. Barlow, H. B., and R. M. Hill. 1963. Selective sensitivity to direction of movement in ganglion cells of the rabbit retina. Science, 139:412.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Cann, M. A. 1961. Rate of aftereffect as a function of texture of the test field in the perception of the negative aftereffect of motion. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Boston University.Google Scholar
  4. Carlson, V. R. 1962. Adaptation in the perception of visual velocity. J. Exp. Psychol., 64:192–197.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Cords, R., and E. T. von Brucke. 1907. Über die Geschwindigkeit des Bewegungsnachbildes. Pfluger Arch. Ges. Physiol., 119:54–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Day, R. H. 1958. On interocular transfer and the central origin of visual aftereffects. Amer. J. Psychol., 71:784–790.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Dvorak, V. 1871. Versuche über die Nachbilder von Reizveranderungen. Sitz. Akad. Wiss. Wien, 61:257–262.Google Scholar
  8. Exner, S. 1876. Über das Sehen von Bewegung und die Theorie des zusammengesetzten Auges. Sitz. Wiener Akad. Wiss., Bd 72, Abstr. III 156–190.Google Scholar
  9. Gallese, A. J. 1956. Spiral aftereffect as a test of organic brain damage. J. Clin. Psychol., 12:254–258.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Grossberg, S. 1969. Embedding fields: a new theory of learning with physiological implications. Mimeographed preprint.Google Scholar
  11. Guilford, J. P. 1936. Psychometric Methods, New York, McGraw-Hill Book Company.Google Scholar
  12. Helmholtz, H. von. 1867. Handbuch der Physiol. Optik. Leipzig, L. Voss.Google Scholar
  13. Holland, H. C. 1965. The Spiral Aftereffect, London, Pergamon Press, Inc.Google Scholar
  14. Holland, H. C. and H. R. Beech. 1958. The spiral aftereffect as a test of brain damage. J. Ment. Sci., 104:466–471.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Hubel, D. H. 1963. The visual cortex of the brain. Sci. Amer., 209:54–62.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Hubel, D. H. and T. N. Wiesel. 1963. Receptive fields of cells in striate cortex of very young, visually inexperienced kittens. J. Neurophysiol., 26:994–1002.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Hutchins, R. M., ed. 1952. Aristotle’s Short Physical Treatises, The Essay on Dreams. Great Books of the Western World. Chicago, Encyclopaedia Britannica, Inc., vol. 1.Google Scholar
  18. Mach, E. 1875. Grundlinie d’Lehre v. d. Bewegungsempfindungen, Leipzig, Engelmann, pp. 59–65.Google Scholar
  19. Michael, C. R. 1969. Retinal processing of visual images. Sci. Amer., 220:105–114.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Pickersgill, M. J. 1964. After-effect of movement in the stimulated and opposite eyes during and after pressure blinding. Nature, 202:833–834.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Plateau, J. 1850. Vierte Notiz über eine neue Sonderbare Anwendung des Verweilens der Eindrucke auf der Netzhaut. Poggendorff’s Annalen, Bd. 80, 287–92. Bulletin de Bruxelles, XVI, II p. 30 and p. 254. Inst., XVIII, no. 835, p. 5. Philos. Paguz., XXXVI, 434–6.Google Scholar
  22. Pribram, K. H. 1969. The neurophysiology of remembering. Sci. Amer., 220:73–86.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Price, A. C, and H. L. Deabler. 1955. Diagnosis of organicity by means of the spiral aftereffect. J. Consult. Psychol., 19:299–302.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Purkinje, J. 1825. Beobachtungen und Versuche zur Physiol, der Sinne, Bd. II, Prague. p. 60.Google Scholar
  25. Scott, T. R. 1964. On the interpretation of “Interocular Transfer.” Percept. Motor Skills, 18:455–466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Scott, T. R. and J. H. Noland. 1965. Some stimulus dimensions of rotating spirals. Psychol. Rev., 72:344–357.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Scott, T. R. and D. Z. Wood. 1966. Retinal anoxia and the locus of the aftereffect of motion. Amer. J. Psychol., 79:435–442.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Scott, T. R., R. A. Bragg, and R. G. Smarr. 1963a. Brain damage diagnosis with the MMG. J. Consult. Psychol., 27:45–53.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Scott, T. R., A. E. Jordan, and D. A. Powell. 1963b. Does visual aftereffect of motion add algebraically to objective motion of the test stimulus? J. Exp. Psychol., 66:500–505.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  30. Scott, T. R., R. A. Bragg, and A. E. Jordan. 1967. Lack of effect of stimulant and depressant drugs on spiral aftereffect. Percept. Motor Skills, 24:1263–1270.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Scott, T. R., A. D. Lavender, R. A. McWhirt, and D. A. Powell. 1966. Directional asymmetry of motion aftereffect. J. Exp. Psychol., 71:806–815.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Stager, P. 1966. An integration of neurological factors in the spiral aftereffect. Canad. J. Psychol., 20:400–406.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Taylor, M. M. 1963a. Tracking the neutralization of seen rotary movement. Percept. Motor Skills, 16:513–519.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Scott, T. R., 1963b. Tracking the decay of the aftereffect of seen rotary movement. Percept. Motor Skills, 16:119–129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Terwilliger, R. F. 1963. Evidence for a relationship between figurai aftereffect and afterimages. Amer. J. Psychol., 76:306–310.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  36. Terwilliger, R. F. 1964. A reply to Scott. Percept. Motor Skills, 18:640.PubMedCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Wohlgemuth, A. 1911. On the aftereffect of seen movement. Brit. J. Psychol., Monogr. Suppl., 1:1–117.Google Scholar
  38. Wundt, W. 1874. Grundzuge der physiol. Psychologie, Leipzig, Engelmann.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media New York 1970

Authors and Affiliations

  • Thomas R. Scott
  • W. Lloyd Milligan
    • 1
  1. 1.Psychological Research LaboratoryVeterans Administration HospitalColumbiaUSA

Personalised recommendations